98-12303. Guidelines for Neurotoxicity Risk Assessment  

  • [Federal Register Volume 63, Number 93 (Thursday, May 14, 1998)]
    [Notices]
    [Pages 26926-26954]
    From the Federal Register Online via the Government Publishing Office [www.gpo.gov]
    [FR Doc No: 98-12303]
    
    
    
    [[Page 26925]]
    
    _______________________________________________________________________
    
    Part III
    
    
    
    
    
    Environmental Protection Agency
    
    
    
    
    
    _______________________________________________________________________
    
    
    
    Guidelines For Neurotoxicity Risk Assessment; Notice
    
    Federal Register / Vol. 63, No. 93 / Thursday, May 14, 1998 / 
    Notices
    
    [[Page 26926]]
    
    
    
    ENVIRONMENTAL PROTECTION AGENCY
    
    [FRL-6011-3]
    RIN 2080-AA08
    
    
    Guidelines for Neurotoxicity Risk Assessment
    
    AGENCY: Environmental Protection Agency.
    
    ACTION: Notice of availability of final Guidelines for Neurotoxicity 
    Risk Assessment.
    
    -----------------------------------------------------------------------
    
    SUMMARY: The U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (EPA) is today 
    publishing in final form a document entitled Guidelines for 
    Neurotoxicity Risk Assessment (hereafter ``Guidelines''). These 
    Guidelines were developed as part of an interoffice guidelines 
    development program by a Technical Panel of the Risk Assessment Forum. 
    The Panel was composed of scientists from throughout the Agency, and 
    selected drafts were peer-reviewed internally and by experts from 
    universities, environmental groups, industry, and other governmental 
    agencies. The Guidelines are based, in part, on recommendations derived 
    from various scientific meetings and workshops on neurotoxicology, from 
    public comments, and from recommendations of the Science Advisory 
    Board. An earlier draft underwent external peer review in a workshop 
    held on June 2-3, 1992, and received internal review by the Risk 
    Assessment Forum. The Risk Assessment Subcommittee of the Committee on 
    the Environment and Natural Resources of Office of Science and 
    Technology Policy reviewed the proposed Guidelines during a meeting 
    held on August 15, 1995. The Guidelines were revised and proposed for 
    public comment on October 4, 1995 (60 FR 52032-52056). The proposed 
    Guidelines were reviewed by the Science Advisory Board on July 18, 
    1996. EPA appreciates the efforts of all participants in the process, 
    and has tried to address their recommendations in these Guidelines.
        This notice describes the scientific basis for concern about 
    exposure to agents that cause neurotoxicity, outlines the general 
    process for assessing potential risk to humans because of environmental 
    contaminants, and addresses Science Advisory Board and public comments 
    on the 1995 Proposed Guidelines for Neurotoxicity Risk Assessment (60 
    FR:52032-52056). These Guidelines are intended to guide Agency 
    evaluation of agents that are suspected to cause neurotoxicity, in line 
    with the policies and procedures established in the statutes 
    administered by the Agency.
    
    DATES: The Guidelines will be effective on April 30, 1998.
    
    ADDRESSES: The Guidelines will be made available in several ways:
        (1) The electronic version will be accessible from EPA's National 
    Center for Environmental Assessment home page on the Internet at http:/
    /www.epa.gov/ncea.
        (2) 3\1/2\'' high-density computer diskettes in WordPerfect format 
    will be available from ORD Publications, Technology Transfer and 
    Support Division, National Risk Management Research Laboratory, 
    Cincinnati, OH; Tel: 513-569-7562; Fax: 513-569-7566. Please provide 
    the EPA No.: EPA/630/R-95/001Fa when ordering.
        (3) This notice contains the full document. Copies of the 
    Guidelines will be available for inspection at EPA headquarters and 
    regional libraries, through the U.S. Government Depository Library 
    program, and for purchase from the National Technical Information 
    Service (NTIS), Springfield, VA; telephone: 703-487-4650, fax: 703-321-
    8547. Please provide the NTIS PB No. (PB98-117831) when ordering.
    
    FOR FURTHER INFORMATION CONTACT: Dr. Hugh A. Tilson, Neurotoxicology 
    Division, National Health and Environmental Effects Research 
    Laboratory, U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, Research Triangle 
    Park, NC 27711, Tel: 919-541-2671; Fax: 919-541-4849; E-mail: 
    tilson.hugh@epamail.epa.gov.
    
    SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION: In its 1983 book Risk Assessment in the 
    Federal Government: Managing the Process, the National Academy of 
    Sciences recommended that Federal regulatory agencies establish 
    ``inference guidelines'' to promote consistency and technical quality 
    in risk assessment, and to ensure that the risk assessment process is 
    maintained as a scientific effort separate from risk management. A task 
    force within EPA accepted that recommendation and requested that Agency 
    scientists begin to develop such guidelines. In 1984, EPA scientists 
    began work on risk assessment guidelines for carcinogenicity, 
    mutagenicity, suspect developmental toxicants, chemical mixtures, and 
    exposure assessment. Following extensive scientific and public review, 
    these first five guidelines were issued on September 24, 1986 (51 FR 
    33992-34054). Since 1986, additional risk assessment guidelines have 
    been proposed, revised, reproposed, and finalized. These guidelines 
    continue the process initiated in 1984. As with other EPA guidelines 
    (e.g., developmental toxicity, 56 FR 63798-63826; exposure assessment, 
    57 FR 22888-22938; and carcinogenicity, 61 FR 17960-18011), EPA will 
    revisit these guidelines as experience and scientific consensus evolve.
        These Guidelines set forth principles and procedures to guide EPA 
    scientists in the conduct of Agency risk assessments and to inform 
    Agency decision makers and the public about these procedures. Policies 
    in this document are intended as internal guidance for EPA. Risk 
    assessors and risk managers at EPA are the primary audience, although 
    these Guidelines may be useful to others outside the Agency. In 
    particular, the Guidelines emphasize that risk assessments will be 
    conducted on a case-by-case basis, giving full consideration to all 
    relevant scientific information. This approach means that Agency 
    experts study scientific information on each chemical under review and 
    use the most scientifically appropriate interpretation to assess risk. 
    The Guidelines also stress that this information will be fully 
    presented in Agency risk assessment documents, and that Agency 
    scientists will identify the strengths and weaknesses of each 
    assessment by describing uncertainties, assumptions, and limitations, 
    as well as the scientific basis and rationale for each assessment. The 
    Guidelines are formulated in part to bridge gaps in risk assessment 
    methodology and data. By identifying these gaps and the importance of 
    the missing information to the risk assessment process, EPA wishes to 
    encourage research and analysis that will lead to new risk assessment 
    methods and data.
    
        Dated: April 30, 1998.
    Carol M. Browner,
    Administrator.
    
    Contents
    
    Part A: Guidelines for Neurotoxicity Risk Assessment
    
    List of Tables
    1. Introduction
        1.1. Organization of These Guidelines
        1.2. The Role of Environmental Agents in Neurotoxicity
        1.3. Neurotoxicity Risk Assessment
        1.4. Assumptions
    2. Definitions and Critical Concepts
    3. Hazard Characterization
        3.1. Neurotoxicological Studies: Endpoints and Their 
    Interpretation
        3.1.1. Human Studies
        3.1.1.1. Clinical Evaluations
        3.1.1.2. Case Reports
        3.1.1.3. Epidemiologic Studies
        3.1.1.4. Human Laboratory Exposure Studies
    
    [[Page 26927]]
    
        3.1.2. Animal Studies
        3.1.2.1. Structural Endpoints of Neurotoxicity
        3.1.2.2. Neurophysiological Endpoints of Neurotoxicity
        3.1.2.3. Neurochemical Endpoints of Neurotoxicity
        3.1.2.4. Behavioral Endpoints of Neurotoxicity
        3.1.3. Other Considerations
        3.1.3.1. Pharmacokinetics
        3.1.3.2. Comparisons of Molecular Structure
        3.1.3.3. Statistical Considerations
        3.1.3.4. In Vitro Data in Neurotoxicology
        3.1.3.5. Neuroendocrine Effects
        3.2. Dose-Response Evaluation
        3.3. Characterization of the Health-Related Database
    4. Quantitative Dose-Response Analysis
        4.1. LOAEL/NOAEL and BMD Determination
        4.2. Determination of the Reference Dose or Reference 
    Concentration
    5. Exposure Assessment
    6. Risk Characterization
        6.1. Overview
        6.2. Integration of Hazard Characterization, Dose-Response 
    Analysis, and Exposure Assessment
        6.3. Quality of the Database and Degree of Confidence in the 
    Assessment
        6.4. Descriptors of Neurotoxicity Risk
        6.4.1. Estimation of the Number of Individuals
        6.4.2. Presentation of Specific Scenarios
        6.4.3. Risk Characterization for Highly Exposed Individuals
        6.4.4. Risk Characterization for Highly Sensitive or Susceptible 
    Individuals
        6.5.5. Other Risk Descriptors
        6.5. Communicating Results
        6.6. Summary and Research Needs
    References
    
    Part B: Response to Science Advisory Board and Public Comments
    
    1. Introduction
    2. Response to Science Advisory Board Comments
    3. Response to Public Comments
    
    List of Tables
    
    Table 1. Examples of possible indicators of a neurotoxic effect
    Table 2. Neurotoxicants and disorders with specific neurological 
    targets
    Table 3. Examples of neurophysiological measures of neurotoxicity
    Table 4. Examples of neurotoxicants with known neurochemical 
    mechanisms
    Table 5. Examples of measures in a representative functional 
    observational battery
    Table 6. Examples of specialized behavioral tests to measure 
    neurotoxicity
    Table 7. Examples of compounds or treatments producing developmental 
    neurotoxicity
    Table 8. Characterization of the health-related database
    
    Part A: Guidelines for Neurotoxicity Risk Assessment
    
    1. Introduction
    
        These Guidelines describe the principles, concepts, and procedures 
    that the U.S. Environmental Protection Agency (EPA) will follow in 
    evaluating data on potential neurotoxicity associated with exposure to 
    environmental toxicants. The Agency's authority to regulate substances 
    that have the potential to interfere with human health is derived from 
    a number of statutes that are implemented through multiple offices 
    within EPA. The procedures outlined here are intended to help develop a 
    sound scientific basis for neurotoxicity risk assessment, promote 
    consistency in the Agency's assessment of toxic effects on the nervous 
    system, and inform others of the approaches used by the Agency in those 
    assessments. This document is not a regulation and is not intended for 
    EPA regulations. The Guidelines set forth current scientific thinking 
    and approaches for conducting and evaluating neurotoxic risk 
    assessments. They are not intended, nor can they be relied upon, to 
    create any rights enforceable by any party in litigation with the 
    United States.
    1.1. Organization of These Guidelines
        This introduction (section 1) summarizes the purpose of these 
    Guidelines within the overall framework of risk assessment at EPA. It 
    also outlines the organization of the guidance and describes several 
    default assumptions to be used in the risk assessment process, as 
    discussed in the recent National Research Council report ``Science and 
    Judgment in Risk Assessment'' (NRC, 1994).
        Section 2 sets forth definitions of particular terms widely used in 
    the field of neurotoxicology. These include ``neurotoxicity'' and 
    ``behavioral alterations.'' Also included in this section are 
    discussions concerning reversible and irreversible effects and direct 
    versus indirect effects.
        Risk assessment is the process by which scientific judgments are 
    made concerning the potential for toxicity in humans. The National 
    Research Council (NRC, 1983) has defined risk assessment as including 
    some or all of the following components (paradigm): hazard 
    identification, dose-response assessment, exposure assessment, and risk 
    characterization. In its 1994 report ``Science and Judgment in Risk 
    Assessment'' the NRC extended its view of the paradigm to include 
    characterization of each component (NRC, 1994). In addition, it noted 
    the importance of an approach that is less fragmented and more 
    holistic, less linear and more interactive, and that deals with 
    recurring conceptual issues that cut across all stages of risk 
    assessment. These Guidelines describe a more interactive approach by 
    organizing the process around the qualitative evaluation of the 
    toxicity data (hazard characterization), the quantitative dose-response 
    analysis, the exposure assessment, and the risk characterization. In 
    these Guidelines, hazard characterization includes deciding whether a 
    chemical has an effect by means of qualitative consideration of dose-
    response relationships, route, and duration of exposure. Determining a 
    hazard often depends on whether a dose-response relationship is present 
    (Kimmel et al., 1990). This approach combines the information important 
    in comparing the toxicity of a chemical with potential human exposure 
    scenarios (section 3). In addition, it avoids the potential for 
    labeling chemicals as ``neurotoxicants'' on a purely qualitative basis. 
    This organization of the risk assessment process is similar to that 
    discussed in the Guidelines for Developmental Toxicity Risk Assessment 
    (56 FR 63798), the main difference being that the quantitative dose-
    response analysis is discussed under a separate section in these 
    Guidelines.
        Hazard characterization involves examining all available 
    experimental animal and human data and the associated doses, routes, 
    timing, and durations of exposure to determine qualitatively if an 
    agent causes neurotoxicity in that species and under what conditions. 
    From the hazard characterization and criteria provided in these 
    Guidelines, the health-related database can be characterized as 
    sufficient or insufficient for use in risk assessment (section 3.3). 
    Combining hazard identification and some aspects of dose-response 
    evaluation into hazard characterization does not preclude the 
    evaluation and use of data for other purposes when quantitative 
    information for setting reference doses (RfDs) and reference 
    concentrations (RfCs) is not available.
        The next step in the dose-response analysis (section 4) is the 
    quantitative analysis, which includes determining the no-observed-
    adverse-effect-level (NOAEL) and/or the lowest-observed-adverse-effect-
    level (LOAEL) for each study and type of effect. Because of the 
    limitations associated with the use of the NOAEL, the Agency is 
    beginning to use an additional approach, the benchmark dose approach 
    (BMD) (Crump, 1984; U.S. EPA, 1995a), for
    
    [[Page 26928]]
    
    more quantitative dose-response evaluation when sufficient data are 
    available. The benchmark dose approach takes into account the 
    variability in the data and the slope of the dose-response curve, and 
    provides a more consistent basis for calculation of the RfD or RfC. If 
    data are considered sufficient for risk assessment, and if 
    neurotoxicity is the effect occurring at the lowest dose level (i.e., 
    the critical effect), an oral or dermal RfD or an inhalation RfC, based 
    on neurotoxic effects, is then derived. This RfD or RfC is derived 
    using the NOAEL or benchmark dose divided by uncertainty factors to 
    account for interspecies differences in response, intraspecies 
    variability, and other factors of study design or the database. A 
    statement of the potential for human risk and the consequences of 
    exposure can come only from integrating the hazard characterization and 
    dose-response analysis with the human exposure estimates in the final 
    risk characterization.
        The section on exposure assessment (section 5) identifies human 
    populations exposed or potentially exposed to an agent, describes their 
    composition and size, and presents the types, magnitudes, frequencies, 
    and durations of exposure to the agent. The exposure assessment 
    provides an estimate of human exposure levels for particular 
    populations from all potential sources.
        In risk characterization (section 6), the hazard characterization, 
    dose-response analysis, and exposure assessment for given populations 
    are combined to estimate some measure of the risk for neurotoxicity. As 
    part of risk characterization, a summary of the strengths and 
    weaknesses of each component of the risk assessment is given, along 
    with major assumptions, scientific judgments and, to the extent 
    possible, qualitative and quantitative estimates of the uncertainties. 
    This characterization of the health-related database is always 
    presented in conjunction with information on the dose, route, duration, 
    and timing of exposure as well as the dose-response analysis including 
    the RfD or RfC. If human exposure estimates are available, the exposure 
    basis used for the risk assessment is clearly described, e.g., highly 
    exposed individuals or highly sensitive or susceptible individuals. The 
    NOAEL may be compared to the various estimates of human exposure to 
    calculate the margin(s) of exposure (MOE). The considerations for 
    judging the acceptability of the MOE are similar to those for 
    determining the appropriate size of the uncertainty factor for 
    calculating the RfD or RfC.
        The Agency recently issued a policy statement and associated 
    guidance for risk characterization (U.S. EPA, 1995b, 1995c), which is 
    currently being implemented throughout EPA. This statement is designed 
    to ensure that critical information from each stage of a risk 
    assessment is used in forming conclusions about risk and that this 
    information is communicated from risk assessors to risk managers 
    (policy makers), from middle to upper management, and from the Agency 
    to the public. Additionally, the policy provides a basis for greater 
    clarity, transparency, reasonableness, and consistency in risk 
    assessments across Agency programs.
        Final neurotoxicity risk assessment guidelines may reflect 
    additional changes in risk characterization practices resulting from 
    implementation activities. Risk assessment is just one component of the 
    regulatory process and defines the potential adverse health 
    consequences of exposure to a toxic agent. The other component, risk 
    management, combines risk assessment with statutory directives 
    regarding socioeconomic, technical, political, and other considerations 
    in order to decide whether to control future exposure to the suspected 
    toxic agent and, if so, the nature and level of control. One major 
    objective of these Guidelines is to help the risk assessor determine 
    whether the experimental animal or human data indicate the potential 
    for a neurotoxic effect. Such information can then be used to 
    categorize evidence that will identify and characterize neurotoxic 
    hazards, as described in section 3.3, Characterization of the Health-
    Related Database, and Table 8 of these Guidelines. Risk management is 
    not dealt with directly in these Guidelines because the basis for 
    decision making goes beyond scientific considerations alone, but the 
    use of scientific information in this process is discussed. For 
    example, the acceptability of the MOE is a risk management decision, 
    but the scientific bases for establishing this value are discussed 
    here.
    1.2. The Role of Environmental Agents in Neurotoxicity
        Chemicals are an integral part of life, with the capacity to 
    improve as well as endanger health. The general population is exposed 
    to chemicals in air, water, foods, cosmetics, household products, and 
    drugs used therapeutically or illicitly. During daily life, a person 
    experiences a multitude of exposures to potentially neuroactive 
    substances, singly and in combination, both synthetic and natural. 
    Levels of exposure vary and may or may not pose a hazard, depending on 
    dose, route, and duration of exposure.
        A link between human exposure to some chemical substances and 
    neurotoxicity has been firmly established (Anger, 1986; OTA, 1990). 
    Because many natural and synthetic chemicals are present in today's 
    environment, there is growing scientific and regulatory interest in the 
    potential for risks to humans from exposure to neurotoxic agents. If 
    sufficient exposure occurs, the effects resulting from such exposures 
    can have a significant adverse impact on human health. It is not known 
    how many chemicals may be neurotoxic in humans (Reiter, 1987). EPA's 
    TSCA inventory of chemical substances manufactured, imported, or 
    processed in the United States includes more than 65,000 substances and 
    is increasing yearly. An overwhelming majority of the materials in 
    commercial use have not been tested for neurotoxic potential (NRC, 
    1984).
        Estimates of the number of chemicals with neurotoxic properties 
    have been made for subsets of substances. For instance, a large 
    percentage of the more than 500 registered active pesticide ingredients 
    affect the nervous system of the target species to varying degrees. Of 
    588 chemicals listed by the American Conference of Governmental 
    Industrial Hygienists, 167 affected the nervous system or behavior at 
    some exposure level (Anger, 1984). Anger (1990) estimated that of the 
    approximately 200 chemicals to which 1 million or more American workers 
    are exposed, more than one-third may have adverse effects on the 
    nervous system if sufficient exposure occurs. Anger (1984) also 
    recognized neurotoxic effects as one of the 10 leading workplace 
    disorders. A number of therapeutic substances, including some 
    anticancer and antiviral agents and abused drugs, can cause adverse or 
    neurotoxicological side effects at therapeutic levels (OTA, 1990). The 
    number of chemicals with neurotoxic potential has been estimated to 
    range from 3% to 28% of all chemicals (OTA, 1990). Thus, estimating the 
    risks of exposure to chemicals with neurotoxic potential is of concern 
    with regard to their overall impact on human health.
    1.3. Neurotoxicity Risk Assessment
        In addition to its primary role in psychological functions, the 
    nervous system controls most, if not all, other bodily processes. It is 
    sensitive to perturbation from various sources and has limited ability 
    to regenerate. There is evidence that even small anatomical, 
    biochemical, or physiological insults to the nervous system may result 
    in
    
    [[Page 26929]]
    
    adverse effects on human health. Therefore, there is a need for 
    consistent guidance on how to evaluate data on neurotoxic substances 
    and assess their potential to cause transient or persistent and direct 
    or indirect effects on human health.
        These Guidelines develop principles and concepts in several areas. 
    They outline the scientific basis for evaluating effects due to 
    exposure to neurotoxicants and discuss principles and methods for 
    evaluating data from human and animal studies on behavior, 
    neurochemistry, neurophysiology, and neuropathology. They also discuss 
    adverse effects on neurological development and function in infants and 
    children following prenatal and perinatal exposure to chemical agents. 
    They outline the methods for calculating reference doses or reference 
    concentrations when neurotoxicity is the critical effect, discuss the 
    availability of alternative mathematical approaches to dose-response 
    analyses, characterize the health-related database for neurotoxicity 
    risk assessment, and discuss the integration of exposure information 
    with results of the dose-response assessment to characterize risks. 
    These Guidelines do not advocate developing reference doses specific 
    for neurotoxicity, but rather support the use of neurotoxicity as one 
    possible endpoint to develop reference doses. EPA offices have 
    published guidelines for neurotoxicity testing in animals (U.S. EPA, 
    1986, 1987, 1988a, 1991a). The testing guidelines address the 
    development of new data for use in risk assessment.
        These neurotoxicity risk assessment guidelines provide the Agency's 
    first comprehensive guidance on the use and interpretation of 
    neurotoxicity data, and are part of the Agency's risk assessment 
    guidelines development process, which was initiated in 1984. As part of 
    its neurotoxicity guidelines development program, EPA has sponsored or 
    participated in several conferences on relevant issues (Tilson, 1990); 
    these and other sources (see references) provide the scientific basis 
    for these Guidelines.
        This guidance is intended for use by Agency risk assessors and is 
    separate and distinct from the recently published document on 
    principles of neurotoxicity risk assessment (U.S. EPA, 1994). The 
    document on principles was prepared under the auspices of the 
    Subcommittee on Risk Assessment of the Federal Coordinating Council for 
    Science, Engineering, and Technology and was not intended to provide 
    specific directives for how neurotoxicity risk assessment should be 
    performed. It is expected that, like other EPA risk assessment 
    guidelines for noncancer endpoints (U.S. EPA, 1991b), this document 
    will encourage research and analysis leading to new risk assessment 
    methods and data, which in turn would be used to revise and improve the 
    Guidelines and better guide Agency risk assessors.
    1.4. Assumptions
        There are a number of unknowns in the extrapolation of data from 
    animal studies to humans. Therefore, a number of default assumptions 
    are made that are generally applied in the absence of data on the 
    relevance of effects to potential human risk. Default assumptions 
    should not be applied indiscriminately. First, all available 
    mechanistic and pharmacokinetic data should be considered. If these 
    data indicate that an alternative assumption is appropriate or if they 
    obviate the need for applying an assumption, such information should be 
    used in risk assessment. For example, research in rats may determine 
    that the neurotoxicity of a chemical is caused by a metabolite. If 
    subsequent research finds that the chemical is metabolized to a lesser 
    degree or not at all in humans, then this information should be used in 
    formulating the default assumptions. The following default assumptions 
    form the basis of the approaches taken in these Guidelines:
        (1) It is assumed that an agent that produces detectable adverse 
    neurotoxic effects in experimental animal studies will pose a potential 
    hazard to humans. This assumption is based on the comparisons of data 
    for known human neurotoxicants (Anger, 1990; Kimmel et al., 1990; 
    Spencer and Schaumburg, 1980), which indicate that experimental animal 
    data are frequently predictive of a neurotoxic effect in humans.
        (2) It is assumed that behavioral, neurophysiological, 
    neurochemical, and neuroanatomical manifestations are of concern. In 
    the past, the tendency has been to consider only neuropathological 
    changes as endpoints of concern. Based on data on agents that are known 
    human neurotoxicants (Anger, 1990; Kimmel et al., 1990; Spencer and 
    Schaumberg, 1980), there is usually at least one experimental species 
    that mimics the types of effects seen in humans, but in other species 
    tested, the neurotoxic effect may be different or absent. For example, 
    certain organophosphate compounds produce a delayed-onset neuropathy in 
    hens similar to that seen in humans, whereas rodents are 
    characteristically insensitive to these compounds. A biologically 
    significant increase in any of the manifestations is considered 
    indicative of an agent's potential for disrupting the structure or 
    function of the human nervous system.
        (3) It is assumed that the neurotoxic effects seen in animal 
    studies may not always be the same as those produced in humans. 
    Therefore, it may be difficult to determine the most appropriate 
    species in terms of predicting specific effects in humans. The fact 
    that every species may not react in the same way is probably due to 
    species-specific differences in maturation of the nervous system, 
    differences in timing of exposure, metabolism, or mechanisms of action.
        (4) It is also assumed that, in the absence of data to the 
    contrary, the most sensitive species is used to estimate human risk. 
    This is based on the assumption that humans are as sensitive as the 
    most sensitive animal species tested. This provides a conservative 
    estimate of sensitivity for added protection to the public. As with 
    other noncancer endpoints, it is assumed that there is a nonlinear 
    dose-response relationship for neurotoxicants. Although there may be a 
    threshold for neurotoxic effects, these are often difficult to 
    determine empirically. Therefore, a nonlinear relationship is assumed 
    to exist for neurotoxicants.
        These assumptions are ``plausibly conservative'' (NRC, 1994) in 
    that they are protective of public health and are also well founded in 
    scientific knowledge about the effects of concern.
    
    2. Definitions and Critical Concepts
    
        This section defines the key terms and concepts that EPA will use 
    in the identification and evaluation of neurotoxicity. The various 
    health effects that fall within the broad classification of 
    neurotoxicity are described and examples are provided. Adverse effects 
    include alterations from baseline or normal conditions that diminish an 
    organism's ability to survive, reproduce, or adapt to the environment. 
    Neurotoxicity is an adverse change in the structure or function of the 
    central and/or peripheral nervous system following exposure to a 
    chemical, physical, or biological agent (Tilson, 1990). Functional 
    neurotoxic effects include adverse changes in somatic/autonomic, 
    sensory, motor, and/or cognitive function. Structural neurotoxic 
    effects are defined as neuroanatomical changes occurring at any level 
    of nervous system organization; functional changes are defined as 
    neurochemical, neurophysiological, or behavioral effects. Chemicals can 
    also be categorized into four classes: Those that act on the central 
    nervous system, the peripheral nerve fibers, the peripheral
    
    [[Page 26930]]
    
    nerve endings, or muscles or other tissues (Albert, 1973). Changes in 
    function can result from toxicity to other specific organ systems, and 
    these indirect changes may be considered adverse. For example, exposure 
    to a high dose of a chemical may cause damage to the liver, resulting 
    in general sickness and a decrease in a functional endpoint such as 
    motor activity. In this case, the change in motor activity could be 
    considered as adverse, but not necessarily neurotoxic. A discussion 
    concerning problems associated with risk assessment of high doses of 
    chemicals in the context of drinking water and health was published by 
    the National Research Council (1986).
        The risk assessor should also know that there are different levels 
    of concern based on the magnitude of effect, duration of exposure, and 
    reversibility of some neurotoxic effects. Neurotoxic effects may be 
    irreversible (the organism cannot return to the state prior to 
    exposure, resulting in a permanent change) or reversible (the organism 
    can return to the pre-exposure condition). Clear or demonstrable 
    irreversible change in either the structure or function of the nervous 
    system causes greater concern than do reversible changes. If neurotoxic 
    effects are observed at some time during the lifespan of the organism 
    but are slowly reversible, the concern is also high. There is lesser 
    concern for effects that are rapidly reversible or ``transient,'' i.e., 
    measured in minutes, hours, or days, and that appear to be associated 
    with the pharmacokinetics of the causal agent and its presence in the 
    body. Reversible changes that occur in the occupational setting or 
    environment, however, may be of high concern if, for example, exposure 
    to a short-acting solvent interferes with operation of heavy equipment 
    in an industrial plant. The context of the exposure should be 
    considered in evaluating reversible effects. Setting of exposure limits 
    is not always associated with the determination of a reference dose, 
    which is based on chronic dosing. Data from acute or subacute dosing 
    can be used for health advisories or in studies involving developmental 
    exposures.
        It should also be noted that the nervous system is known for its 
    reserve capacity (Tilson and Mitchell, 1983). That is, repeated insult 
    to the nervous system could lead to an adaptation. There are, however, 
    limits to this capacity, and when these limits are exceeded, further 
    exposure could lead to frank manifestations of neurotoxicity at the 
    structural or functional level. The risk assessor should be aware that 
    once damaged, neurons, particularly in the central nervous system, have 
    a limited capacity for regeneration. Reversibility of effects resulting 
    from cell death or from the destruction of cell processes may represent 
    an activation of repair capacity, decreasing future potential 
    adaptability. Therefore, even reversible neurotoxic changes should be 
    of concern. Evidence of progressive effects (those that continue to 
    worsen even after the causal agent has been removed), delayed-onset 
    effects (those that occur at a time distant from the last contact with 
    the causal agent), residual effects (those that persist beyond a 
    recovery period), or latent effects (those that become evident only 
    after an environmental challenge or aging) have a high level of 
    concern.
        Environmental challenges can include stress, increased physical or 
    cognitive workload, pharmacological manipulations, and nutritional 
    deficiency or excess. Evidence for reversibility may depend on the 
    region of the nervous system affected, the chemical involved, and 
    organismic factors such as the age of the exposed population. Some 
    regions of the nervous system, such as peripheral nerves, have a high 
    capacity for regeneration, while regions in the brain such as the 
    hippocampus are known for their ability to compensate or adapt to 
    neurotoxic insult. For example, compensation is likely to be seen with 
    solvents (e.g., n-hexane) that produce peripheral neuropathy because of 
    the repair capacity of the peripheral nerve. In addition, tolerance to 
    some cholinergic effects of cholinesterase-inhibiting compounds may be 
    due to compensatory down-regulation of muscarinic receptors. Younger 
    individuals may have more capacity to adapt than older individuals, 
    suggesting that the aged may be at greater risk to neurotoxic exposure.
        Neurotoxic effects can be observed at various levels of 
    organization of the nervous system, including neurochemical, 
    anatomical, physiological, or behavioral. At the neurochemical level, 
    for example, an agent that causes neurotoxicity might inhibit 
    macromolecule or transmitter synthesis, alter the flow of ions across 
    cellular membranes, or prevent release of neurotransmitter from the 
    nerve terminals. Anatomical changes may include alterations of the cell 
    body, the axon, or the myelin sheath. At the physiological level, a 
    chemical might change the thresholds for neural activation or reduce 
    the speed of neurotransmission. Behavioral alterations can include 
    significant changes in sensations of sight, hearing, or touch; 
    alterations in simple or complex reflexes and motor functions; 
    alterations in cognitive functions such as learning, memory, or 
    attention; and changes in mood, such as fear or rage, disorientation as 
    to person, time, or place, or distortions of thinking and feeling, such 
    as delusions and hallucinations. At present, relatively few neurotoxic 
    syndromes have been thoroughly characterized in terms of the initial 
    neurochemical change, structural alterations, physiological 
    consequence, and behavioral effects. Knowledge of exact mechanisms of 
    action is not, however, necessary to conclude that a chemically induced 
    change is a neurotoxic effect.
        Neurotoxic effects can be produced by chemicals that do not require 
    metabolism prior to interacting with their sites in the nervous system 
    (primary neurotoxic agents) or those that require metabolism prior to 
    interacting with their sites (secondary neurotoxic agents). Chemically 
    induced neurotoxic effects can be direct (due to an agent or its 
    metabolites acting directly on sites in the nervous system) or indirect 
    (due to agents or metabolites that produce their effects primarily by 
    interacting with sites outside the nervous system). For example, 
    excitatory amino acids such as domoic acid damage specific neurons 
    directly by activating excitatory amino acid receptors in the nervous 
    system, whereas carbon monoxide decreases oxygen availability, which 
    can indirectly kill neurons. Other examples of indirect effects include 
    cadmium-induced spasms in blood vessels supplying the nervous system, 
    dichloroacetate-induced perturbation of metabolic pathways, and 
    chemically induced alterations in skeletomuscular function or structure 
    and effects on the endocrine system. Professional judgment may be 
    required in making determinations about direct versus indirect effects.
        The interpretation of data as indicative of a potential neurotoxic 
    effect involves the evaluation of the validity of the database. This 
    approach and these terms have been adapted from the literature on human 
    psychological testing (Sette, 1987; Sette and MacPhail, 1992), where 
    they have long been used to evaluate the level of confidence in 
    different measures of intelligence or other abilities, aptitudes, or 
    feelings. There are four principal questions that should be addressed: 
    whether the effects result from exposure (content validity); whether 
    the effects are adverse or toxicologically significant (construct 
    validity); whether there are correlative measures among behavioral, 
    physiological, neurochemical, and
    
    [[Page 26931]]
    
    morphological endpoints (concurrent validity); and whether the effects 
    are predictive of what will happen under various conditions (predictive 
    validity). Addressing these issues can provide a useful framework for 
    evaluating either human or animal studies or the weight of evidence for 
    a chemical (Sette, 1987; Sette and MacPhail, 1992). The next sections 
    indicate the extent to which chemically induced changes can be 
    interpreted as providing evidence of neurotoxicity.
    
    3. Hazard Characterization
    
    3.1. Neurotoxicological Studies: Endpoints and Their Interpretation
        The qualitative characterization of neurotoxic hazard can be based 
    on either human or animal data (Anger, 1984; Reiter, 1987; U.S. EPA, 
    1994). Such data can result from accidental, inappropriate, or 
    controlled experimental exposures. This section describes many of the 
    general and some of the specific characteristics of human studies and 
    reports of neurotoxicity. It then describes some features of animal 
    studies of neuroanatomical, neurochemical, neurophysiological, and 
    behavioral effects relevant to risk assessment. The process of 
    characterizing the sufficiency or insufficiency of neurotoxic effects 
    for risk assessment is described in section 3.3. Additional sources of 
    information relevant to hazard characterization, such as comparisons of 
    molecular structure among compounds and in vitro screening methods, are 
    also discussed.
        The hazard characterization should:
        a. Identify strengths and limitations of the database:
         Epidemiological studies (case reports, cross-sectional, 
    case-control, cohort, or human laboratory exposure studies);
         Animal studies (including structural or neuropathological, 
    neurochemical, neurophysiological, behavioral or neurological, or 
    developmental endpoints).
        b. Evaluate the validity of the database:
         Content validity (effects result from exposure);
         Construct validity (effects are adverse or toxicologically 
    significant);
         Concurrent validity (correlative measures among 
    behavioral, physiological, neurochemical, or morphological endpoints);
         Predictive validity (effects are predictive of what will 
    happen under various conditions).
        c. Identify and describe key toxicological studies.
        d. Describe the type of effects:
         Structural (neuroanatomical alternations);
         Functional (neurochemical, neurophysiological, behavioral 
    alterations).
        e. Describe the nature of the effects (irreversible, reversible, 
    transient, progressive, delayed, residual, or latent).
        f. Describe how much is known about how (through what biological 
    mechanism) the chemical produces adverse effects.
        g. Discuss other health endpoints of concern.
        h. Comment on any nonpositive data in humans or animals.
        I. Discuss the dose-response data (epidemiological or animal) 
    available for further dose-response analysis.
        j. Discuss the route, level, timing, and duration of exposure in 
    studies demonstrating neurotoxicity as compared to expected human 
    exposures.
        k. Summarize the hazard characterization:
         Confidence in conclusions;
         Alternative conclusions also supported by the data;
         Significant data gaps; and
         Highlights of major assumptions.
    3.1.1. Human Studies
        It is well established that information from the evaluation of 
    human exposure can identify neurotoxic hazards (Anger and Johnson, 
    1985; Anger, 1990). Prominent among historical episodes of 
    neurotoxicity in human populations are the outbreaks of methylmercury 
    poisoning in Japan and Iraq and the neurotoxicity seen in miners of 
    metals, including mercury, manganese, and lead (Carson et al., 1987; 
    Silbergeld and Percival, 1987; OTA, 1990). In the past decade, lead 
    poisoning in children has been a prominent issue of concern (Silbergeld 
    and Percival, 1987). Neurotoxicity in humans has been studied and 
    reviewed for many pesticides (Hayes, 1982; NRDC, 1989; Ecobichon and 
    Joy, 1982; Ecobichon et al., 1990). Organochlorines, organophosphates, 
    carbamates, pyrethroids, certain fungicides, and some fumigants are all 
    known neurotoxicants. They may pose occupational risks to manufacturing 
    and formulation workers, pesticide applicators and farm workers, and 
    consumers through home application or consumption of residues in foods. 
    Families of workers may also be exposed by transport into the home from 
    workers' clothing. Data on humans can come from a number of sources, 
    including clinical evaluations, case reports, epidemiologic studies, 
    and human laboratory exposure studies. A more extensive description of 
    issues concerning human neurotoxicology and risk assessment has been 
    published elsewhere (U.S. EPA, 1993). A review of the types of tests 
    used to assess cognitive and neurological function in children, in 
    addition to a discussion of methodological issues in the design of 
    prospective, longitudinal studies of developmental neurotoxicity in 
    humans, has recently been published (Jacobson and Jacobson, 1996). 
    Stanton and Spear (1990) reviewed assessment measures used in 
    developmental neurotoxicology for their comparability in humans and 
    laboratory animals and their ability to detect comparable adverse 
    effects across species. At the level of the various functional 
    assessments for sensory, motivational, cognitive and motor function, 
    and social behavior, there was good agreement across species among the 
    neurotoxic agents reviewed.
    3.1.1.1. Clinical Evaluations
        Clinical methods are used extensively in neurology and 
    neuropsychology to evaluate patients suspected of having neurotoxicity. 
    An array of examiner-administered and paper-and-pencil tasks are used 
    to assess sensory, motor, cognitive, and affective functions and 
    personality states/traits. Neurobehavioral data are synthesized with 
    information from neurophysiological studies and medical history to 
    derive a working diagnosis. Brain functional imaging techniques based 
    on magnetic resonance imaging or emission tomography may also be useful 
    in helping diagnose neurodegenerative disorders following chemical 
    exposures in humans (Omerand et al., 1994; Callender et al., 1994). 
    Clinical diagnostic approaches have provided a rich conceptual 
    framework for understanding the functions (and malfunctions) of the 
    central and peripheral nervous systems and have formed the basis for 
    the development of methods for measuring the behavioral expression of 
    nervous system disorders. Human neurobehavioral toxicology has borrowed 
    heavily from neurology and neuropsychology for concepts of nervous 
    system impairment and functional assessment methods. Neurobehavioral 
    toxicology has adopted the neurologic/neuropsychologic model, using 
    adverse changes in behavioral function to assist in identifying 
    chemical-or drug-induced changes in nervous system processes.
        Neurological and neuropsychological methods have long been employed 
    to identify the adverse health effects of environmental workplace 
    exposures (Sterman and Schaumburg, 1980).
    
    [[Page 26932]]
    
    Peripheral neuropathies (with sensory and motor disturbances), 
    encephalopathies, organic brain syndromes, extrapyramidal syndromes, 
    demyelination, autonomic changes, and dementia are well-characterized 
    consequences of acute and chronic exposure to chemical agents. The 
    range of exposure conditions that produce clinical signs of 
    neurotoxicity also has been defined by these clinical methods. It is 
    very important to make external/internal dose measurements in humans to 
    determine the actual dose(s) that can cause unwanted effects.
        Aspects of the neurological examination approach limit its 
    usefulness for neurotoxicological risk assessment. Information obtained 
    from the neurological exam is mostly qualitative and descriptive rather 
    than quantitative. Estimates of the severity of functional impairment 
    can be reliably placed into only three or four categories (for example, 
    mild, moderate, severe). Much of the assessment depends on the 
    subjective judgment of the examiner. For example, the magnitude and 
    symmetry of muscle strength are often judged by having the patient push 
    against the resistance of the examiner's hands. The endpoints are 
    therefore the absolute and relative amount of muscle load sensed by the 
    examiner in his or her arms.
        Compared with other methods, the neurological exam may be less 
    sensitive in detecting early neurotoxicity in peripheral sensory and 
    motor nerves. While clinicians' judgments are equal in sensitivity to 
    quantitative methods in assessing the amplitude of tremor, tremor 
    frequency is poorly quantified by clinicians. Thus, important aspects 
    of the clinical neurologic exam may be insufficiently quantified and 
    lack sufficient sensitivity for detecting early neurobehavioral 
    toxicity produced by environmental or workplace exposure conditions. 
    However, a neurological evaluation of persons with documented 
    neurobehavioral impairment would be helpful for identifying nonchemical 
    causes of neurotoxicity, such as diabetes and cardiovascular 
    insufficiency.
        Administration of a neuropsychological battery also requires a 
    trained technician, and interpretation requires a trained and 
    experienced neuropsychologist. Depending on the capabilities of the 
    patient, 2 to 4 hours may be needed to administer a full battery; 1 
    hour may be needed for the shorter screening versions. These practical 
    considerations may limit the usefulness of neuropsychological 
    assessment in large field studies of suspected neurotoxicity.
        In addition to logistical problems in administration and 
    interpretation, neuropsychological batteries and neurological exams 
    share two disadvantages with respect to neurotoxicity risk assessment. 
    First, neurological exams and neuropsychological test batteries are 
    designed to confirm and classify functional problems in individuals 
    selected on the basis of signs and symptoms identified by the patient, 
    family, or other health professionals. Their usefulness in detecting 
    low base-rate impairment in workers or the general population is 
    generally thought to be limited, decreasing the usefulness of clinical 
    assessment approaches for epidemiologic risk assessment.
        Second, neurological exams and neuropsychological test batteries 
    were developed to assess the functional correlates of the most common 
    forms of nervous system dysfunction: brain trauma, focal lesions, and 
    degenerative conditions. The clinical tests were validated against 
    these neurological disease states. With a few notable exceptions, 
    chemicals are not believed to produce impairment similar to that from 
    trauma or lesions; neurotoxic effects are more similar to the effects 
    of degenerative disease. There has been insufficient research to 
    demonstrate which tests designed to assess functional expression of 
    neurologic disease are useful in characterizing the modes of central 
    nervous system impairment produced by chemical agents and drugs.
        It should be noted that alternative approaches are available that 
    avoid many of the limitations of clinical and neurological and 
    traditional neuropsychological methods. Computerized behavioral 
    assessment systems designed for field testing of populations exposed to 
    chemicals in the community or workplace have been developed during the 
    past decade. The most widely used system is the Neurobehavioral 
    Evaluation System (NES) developed by Baker et al. (1985). Advantages of 
    computerized tests include (1) standardized administration to eliminate 
    intertester variability and minimize subject-experimenter interaction; 
    (2) automated data collection and scoring, which is faster, easier, and 
    less error-prone than traditional methods; and (3) test administration 
    requires minimal training and experience. NES tests have proven 
    sensitive to a variety of solvents, metals, and pesticides (Otto, 
    1992). Computerized systems available for human neurotoxicity testing 
    are critically reviewed in Anger et al. (1996).
    3.1.1.2. Case Reports
        The first type of human data available is often the case report or 
    case series, which can identify cases of a disease and are reported by 
    clinicians or discerned through active or passive surveillance, usually 
    in the workplace. However, case reports involving a single neurotoxic 
    agent, although informative, are rare in the literature; for example, 
    farmers are likely to be exposed to a wide variety of potentially 
    neurotoxic pesticides. Careful case histories assist in identifying 
    common risk factors, especially when the association between the 
    exposure and disease is strong, the mode of action of the agent is 
    biologically plausible, and clusters occur in a limited period of time.
        Case reports can be obtained more quickly than more complex 
    studies. Case reports of acute high-level exposure to a toxicant can be 
    useful for identifying signs and symptoms that may also apply to lower 
    exposure. Case reports can also be useful when corroborating 
    epidemiological data are available.
    3.1.1.3. Epidemiologic Studies
        Epidemiology has been defined as ``the study of the distributions 
    and determinants of disease and injuries in human populations'' 
    (Mausner and Kramer, 1985). Knowing the frequency of illness in groups 
    and the factors that influence the distribution is the tool of 
    epidemiology that allows the evaluation of causal inference with the 
    goal of prevention and cure of disease (Friedlander and Hearn, 1980). 
    Epidemiologic studies are a useful means of evaluating the effects of 
    neurotoxic substances on human populations, particularly if effects of 
    exposure are cumulative or exposures are repeated. Such studies are 
    less useful in cases of acute exposure, where the effects are short-
    term. Frequently, determining the precise dose or exposure 
    concentration in epidemiological studies can be difficult.
    3.1.1.3.1. Cross-Sectional Studies.
        In cross-sectional studies or surveys, both the disease and 
    suspected risk factors are ascertained at the same time, and the 
    findings are useful in generating hypotheses. A group of people are 
    interviewed, examined, and tested at a single point in time to 
    ascertain a relationship between a disease and a neurotoxic exposure. 
    This study design does not allow the investigator to determine whether 
    the disease or the exposure came first, rendering it less useful in 
    estimating risk. These studies are intermediate in cost and time
    
    [[Page 26933]]
    
    required to complete compared with case reports and more complex 
    analytical studies, but should be augmented with additional data.
    3.1.1.3.2. Case-Control (Retrospective) Studies.
        Last (1986) defines a case-control study as one that ``starts with 
    the identification of persons with the disease (or other outcome 
    variable) of interest, and a suitable control population (comparison, 
    reference group) of persons without the disease.'' He states that the 
    relationship of an ``attribute'' to the disease is measured by 
    comparing the diseased with the nondiseased with regard to how 
    frequently the attribute is present in each of the groups. The cases 
    are assembled from a population of persons with and without exposure, 
    and the comparison group is selected from the same population; the 
    relative distribution of the potential risk factor (exposure) in both 
    groups is evaluated by computing an odds ratio that serves as an 
    estimate of the strength of the association between the disease and the 
    potential risk factor. The statistical significance of the ratio is 
    determined by calculating a p-value and is used to approximate relative 
    risk.
        The case-control approach to the study of potential neurotoxicants 
    in the environment provides a great deal of useful information for the 
    risk assessor. In his textbook, Valciukas (1991) notes that the case-
    control approach is the strategy of choice when no other environmental 
    or biological indicator of neurotoxic exposure is available. He further 
    states: ``Considering the fact that for the vast majority of neurotoxic 
    chemical compounds, no objective biological indicators of exposure are 
    available (or if they are, their half-life is too short to be of any 
    practical value), the case-control paradigm is a widely accepted 
    strategy for the assessment of toxic causation.'' The case-control 
    study design, however, can be very susceptible to bias. The potential 
    sources of bias are numerous and can be specific to a particular study. 
    Many of these biases also can be present in cross-sectional studies. 
    For example, recall bias or faulty recall of information by study 
    subjects in a questionnaire-based study can distort the results. 
    Analysis of the case-comparison study design assumes that the selected 
    cases are representative persons with the disease--either all cases 
    with the disease or a representative sample of them have been 
    ascertained. It further assumes that the control or comparison group is 
    representative of the nonexposed population (or that the prevalence of 
    the characteristic under study is the same in the control group as in 
    the general population). Failure to satisfy these assumptions may 
    result in selection bias that may invalidate study results.
        An additional source of bias in case-control studies is the 
    presence of confounding variables, i.e., factors known to be associated 
    with the exposure and causally related to the disease under study. 
    These should be controlled, either in the design of the study by 
    matching cases to controls on the basis of the confounding factor, or 
    in the analysis of the data by using statistical techniques such as 
    stratification or regression. Matching requires time to identify an 
    adequate number of potential controls to distinguish those with the 
    proper characteristics, while statistical control of confounding 
    factors requires a larger study.
        The definition of exposure is critical in epidemiologic studies. In 
    occupational settings, exposure assessment often is based on the job 
    assignment of the study subjects, but can be more precise if detailed 
    company records allow the development of exposure profiles. Positive 
    results from a properly controlled retrospective study should weigh 
    heavily in the risk assessment process.
    3.1.1.3.3. Cohort (Prospective, Follow-Up) Studies.
        In a prospective study design, a healthy group of people is 
    assembled and followed forward in time and observed for the development 
    of dysfunction. Such studies are invaluable for determining the time 
    course for development of dysfunction (e.g., follow-up studies 
    performed in various cities on the effects of lead on child 
    development). This approach allows the direct estimate of risks 
    attributed to a particular exposure, since toxic incidence rates in the 
    cohort can be determined. Prospective study designs also allow the 
    study of chronic effects of exposure. One major strength of the cohort 
    design is that it allows the calculation of rates to determine the 
    excess risk associated with an exposure. Also, biases are reduced by 
    obtaining information before the disease develops. This approach, 
    however, can be very time-consuming and costly.
        In cohort studies information bias can be introduced when 
    individuals provide distorted information about their health because 
    they know their exposure status and may have been told of the expected 
    health effects of the exposure under study. More credence should be 
    given to those studies in which both observer and subject bias are 
    carefully controlled (e.g., double-blind studies).
        A special type of cohort study is the retrospective cohort study, 
    in which the investigator goes back in time to select the study groups 
    and traces them over time, often to the present. The studies usually 
    involve specially exposed groups and have provided much assistance in 
    estimating risks due to occupational exposures. Occupational 
    retrospective cohort studies rely on company records of past and 
    current employees that include information on the dates of employment, 
    age at employment, date of departure, and whether diseased (or dead in 
    the case of mortality studies). Workers can then be classified by 
    duration and degree of exposure. Positive or negative results from a 
    properly controlled prospective study should weigh heavily in the risk 
    assessment process.
    3.1.1.4. Human Laboratory Exposure Studies
        Neurotoxicity assessment has an advantage not afforded to the 
    evaluation of other toxic endpoints, such as cancer or reproductive 
    toxicity, in that the effects of some chemicals are short in duration 
    and reversible. This makes it ethically possible to perform human 
    laboratory exposure studies and obtain data relevant to the risk 
    assessment process. Information from experimental human exposure 
    studies has been used to set occupational exposure limits, mostly for 
    organic solvents that can be inhaled. Laboratory exposure studies have 
    contributed to risk assessment and the setting of exposure limits for 
    several solvents and other chemicals with acute reversible effects.
        Human exposure studies sometimes offer advantages over 
    epidemiologic field studies. Combined with appropriate sampling of 
    biological fluids (urine or blood), it is possible to calculate body 
    concentrations, examine toxicokinetics, and identify metabolites. 
    Bioavailability, elimination, dose-related changes in metabolic 
    pathways, individual variability, time course of effects, interactions 
    between chemicals, and interactions between chemical and environmental/
    biobehavioral processes (stressors, workload/respiratory rate) are 
    factors that are generally easier to collect under controlled 
    conditions.
        Other goals of laboratory studies include the in-depth 
    characterization of effects, the development of new assessment methods, 
    and the examination of the sensitivity, specificity, and reliability of 
    neurobehavioral assessment methods across chemical classes. The 
    laboratory is the most appropriate setting for the
    
    [[Page 26934]]
    
    study of environmental and biobehavioral variables that affect the 
    action of chemical agents. The effects of ambient temperature, task 
    difficulty, rate of ongoing behavior, conditioning variables, 
    tolerance/sensitization, sleep deprivation, motivation, and so forth 
    are sometimes studied.
        From a methodological standpoint, human laboratory studies can be 
    divided into two categories: between-subjects and within-subjects 
    designs. In the former, the neurobehavioral performance of exposed 
    volunteers is compared with that of nonexposed participants. In the 
    latter, preexposure performance is compared with neurobehavioral 
    function under the influence of the chemical or drug. Within-subjects 
    designs have the advantage of requiring fewer participants, eliminating 
    individual differences as a source of variability, and controlling for 
    chronic mediating variables, such as caffeine use and educational 
    achievement. A disadvantage of the within-subjects design is that 
    neurobehavioral tests must be administered more than once. Practice on 
    many neurobehavioral tests often leads to improved performance that may 
    confound the effect of the chemical/drug. There should be a sufficient 
    number of test sessions in the pre-exposure phase to allow performance 
    on all tests to achieve a relatively stable baseline level.
        Participants in laboratory exposure studies may have been recruited 
    from populations of persons already exposed to the chemical/drug or 
    from chemical-naive populations. Although the use of exposed volunteers 
    has ethical advantages, can mitigate against novelty effects, and 
    allows evaluation of tolerance/sensitization, finding an accessible 
    exposed population in reasonable proximity to the laboratory can be 
    difficult. Chemical-naive participants are more easily recruited but 
    may differ significantly in important characteristics from a 
    representative sample of exposed persons. Chemical-naive volunteers are 
    often younger, healthier, and better educated than the populations 
    exposed environmentally, in the workplace, or pharmacotherapeutically.
        Compared with workplace and environmental exposures, laboratory 
    exposure conditions can be controlled more precisely, but exposure 
    periods are much shorter. Generally only one or two relatively pure 
    chemicals are studied for several hours, whereas the population of 
    interest may be exposed to multiple chemicals containing impurities for 
    months or years. Laboratory studies are therefore better at identifying 
    and characterizing effects with acute onset and the selective effects 
    of pure agents. In all cases, the potential for participant bias should 
    be as carefully controlled for as possible. Even the consent form can 
    lead to participant bias, as toxic effects have been reported in some 
    individuals who were warned of such effects in an informed consent 
    form. In addition, double-blind studies have been shown to provide some 
    control for observer bias that may occur in single-blind studies. More 
    credence should be given to those studies in which both observer and 
    subject bias are carefully controlled (Benignus, 1993).
        A test battery that examines multiple neurobehavioral functions may 
    be more useful for screening and the initial characterization of acute 
    effects. Selected neurobehavioral tests that measure a limited number 
    of functions in multiple ways may be more useful for elucidating 
    mechanisms or validating specific effects.
        Both chemical and behavioral control procedures are valuable for 
    examining the specificity of the effects. A concordant effect among 
    different measures of the same neurobehavioral function (e.g., reaction 
    time) and a lack of effect on some other measures of psychomotor 
    function (e.g., untimed manual dexterity) would increase the confidence 
    in a selective effect on motor speed and not on attention or another 
    nonspecific motor function. Likewise, finding concordant effects among 
    similar chemical or drug classes along with different effects from 
    dissimilar classes would support the specificity of chemical effect. 
    For example, finding that the effects of a solvent were similar to 
    those of ethanol but not caffeine would support the specificity of 
    solvent effects on a given measure of neurotoxicity.
    3.1.2. Animal Studies
        This section provides an overview of the major types of endpoints 
    that may be evaluated in animal neurotoxicity studies, describes the 
    kinds of effects that may be observed and some of the tests used to 
    detect and quantify these effects, and provides guidance for 
    interpreting data. Compared with human studies, animal studies are more 
    often available for specific chemicals, provide more precise exposure 
    information, and control environmental factors better (Anger, 1984). 
    For these reasons, risk assessments tend to rely heavily on animal 
    studies.
        Many tests that can measure some aspect of neurotoxicity have been 
    used in the field of neurobiology in the past 50 years. The Office of 
    Prevention, Pesticides and Toxic Substances (OPPTS) has published 
    animal testing guidelines that were developed in cooperation with the 
    Office of Research and Development (U.S. EPA, 1991a). While the test 
    endpoints included in the 1991 document serve as a convenient focus for 
    this section, there are many other endpoints for which there are no 
    current EPA guidelines. The goal of the current document is to provide 
    a framework for interpreting data collected in tests frequently used by 
    neurotoxicologists.
        Five categories of endpoints will be described: structural or 
    neuropathological, neurophysiological, neurochemical, behavioral, and 
    developmental. Table 1 lists a number of endpoints in each of these 
    categories. It is imperative for the risk assessor to understand that 
    the interpretation of the indicators listed in Table 1 as neurotoxic 
    effects is dependent on the dose at which such changes occur and the 
    possibility that damage to other organ systems may contribute to or 
    cause such changes indirectly.
    
        Table 1.--Examples of Possible Indicators of a Neurotoxic Effect    
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
                                                                            
    -------------------------------------------------------------------------
    Structural or neuropathological endpoints:                              
        Gross changes in morphology, including brain weight.                
        Histologic changes in neurons or glia (neuronopathy, axonopathy,    
         myelinopathy).                                                     
    Neurochemical endpoints:                                                
        Alterations in synthesis, release, uptake, degradation of           
         neurotransmitters.                                                 
        Alterations in second-messenger-associated signal transduction.     
        Alterations in membrane-bound enzymes regulating neuronal activity. 
        Inhibition and aging of neuropathy enzyme.                          
        Increases in glial fibrillary acidic protein in adults.             
    Neurophysiological endpoints:                                           
        Change in velocity, amplitude, or refractory period of nerve        
         conduction.                                                        
    
    [[Page 26935]]
    
                                                                            
        Change in latency or amplitude of sensory-evoked potential.         
        Change in electroencephalographic pattern.                          
    Behavioral and neurological endpoints:                                  
        Increases or decreases in motor activity.                           
        Changes in touch, sight, sound, taste, or smell sensations.         
        Changes in motor coordination, weakness, paralysis, abnormal        
         movement or posture, tremor, ongoing performance.                  
        Absence or decreased occurrence, magnitude, or latency of           
         sensorimotor reflex.                                               
        Altered magnitude of neurological measurement, including grip       
         strength, hindlimb splay.                                          
        Seizures.                                                           
        Changes in rate or temporal patterning of schedule-controlled       
         behavior.                                                          
        Changes in learning, memory, and attention.                         
    Developmental endpoints:                                                
        Chemically induced changes in the time of appearance of behaviors   
         during development.                                                
        Chemically induced changes in the growth or organization of         
         structural or neurochemical elements.                              
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
    
    3.1.2.1. Structural Endpoints of Neurotoxicity
        Structural endpoints are typically defined as neuropathological 
    changes evident by gross observation or light microscopy, although most 
    neurotoxic changes will be detectable only at the light microscopic 
    level. Gross changes in morphology can include discrete or widespread 
    lesions in nerve tissue. A change in brain weight is considered to be a 
    biologically significant effect. This is true regardless of changes in 
    body weight, because brain weight is generally protected during 
    undernutrition or weight loss, unlike many other organs or tissues. It 
    is inappropriate to express brain weight changes as a ratio of body 
    weight and thereby dismiss changes in absolute brain weight. Changes in 
    brain weight are a more reliable indicator of alteration in brain 
    structure than are measurements of length or width in fresh brain, 
    because there is little historical data in the toxicology literature.
        Neurons are composed of a neuronal body, axon, and dendritic 
    processes. Various types of neuropathological lesions may be classified 
    according to the site where they occur (Spencer and Schaumburg, 1980; 
    WHO, 1986; Krinke, 1989; Griffin, 1990). Neurotoxicant-induced lesions 
    in the central or peripheral nervous system may be classified as a 
    neuronopathy (changes in the neuronal cell body), axonopathy (changes 
    in the axons), myelinopathy (changes in the myelin sheaths), or nerve 
    terminal degeneration. Nerve terminal degeneration represents a very 
    subtle change that may not be detected by routine histopathology, but 
    requires detection by special procedures such as silver staining or 
    neurotransmitter-specific immunohistochemistry. For axonopathies, a 
    more precise location of the changes may also be described (i.e., 
    proximal, central, or distal axonopathy). In the case of some 
    developmental exposures, a neurotoxic chemical might delay or 
    accelerate the differentiation or proliferation of cells or cell types. 
    Alteration in the axonal termination site might also occur with 
    exposure. In an aged population, exposure to some neurotoxicants might 
    accelerate the normal loss of neurons associated with aging (Reuhl, 
    1991). In rare cases, neurotoxic agents have been reported to produce 
    neuropathic conditions resembling neurodegenerative disorders, such as 
    Parkinson's disease, in humans (WHO, 1986). Table 2 lists examples of 
    such neurotoxic chemicals, their putative site of action, the type of 
    neuropathology produced, and the disorder or condition that each 
    typifies. Inclusion of any chemical in any of the following tables is 
    for illustrative purposes, i.e., it has been reported that the chemical 
    will produce a neurotoxic effect at some dose; any individual chemical 
    listed may also adversely affect other organs at lower doses. It is 
    important that the severity of each structural union be graded 
    objectively and the grading criteria reported.
    
                        Table 2.--Neurotoxicants and Disorders With Specific Neurological Targets                   
    ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
                                                                                                  Corresponding     
               Site of action                Neurotoxic change       Neurotoxic chemical        neurodegenerative   
                                                                                                    disorder        
    ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
    Neuron cell body....................  Neuronopathy...........  Methylmercury..........  Minamata disease.       
                                                                   Quinolinic acid........  Huntington's disease.   
                                                                   3-Acetylpyridine.......  Cerebellar ataxia.      
    Nerve terminal......................  Terminal destruction...  1-Methyl-4-phenyl 1,2,.  Parkinson's disease.    
                                                                   3,6-tetrahydro-........                          
                                                                   pyridine (MPTP)                                  
                                                                    (dopaminergic).                                 
    Schwann cell myelin.................  Myelinopathy...........  Hexachlorophene........  Congenital              
                                                                                             hypomyelinogenesis.    
    Centra-peripheral distal axon.......  Distal axonopathy......  Acrylamide, carbon       Peripheral neuropathy.  
                                                                    disulfide, n-hexane.                            
    Central axons.......................  Central axonopathy.....  Clioquinol.............  Subacute                
                                                                                             myeloopticoneuro-pathy.
    Proximal axon.......................  Proximal axonopathy....  B,B'-                    Motor neuron disease.   
                                                                    Iminodipropionitrile.                           
    ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
    
        Alterations in the structure of the nervous system (i.e., 
    neuronopathy, axonopathy, myelinopathy, terminal degeneration) are 
    regarded as evidence of a neurotoxic effect. The risk assessor should 
    note that pathological changes in many cases require time for the 
    perturbation to become observable, especially with evaluation at the 
    light microscopic level. Neuropathological studies should control for 
    potential differences in the area(s) and section(s) of the nervous 
    system sampled; in the age, sex, and body weight of the subject; and in 
    fixation artifacts (WHO, 1986). Concern for the structural integrity of 
    nervous system tissues derives from
    
    [[Page 26936]]
    
    their functional specialization and lack of regenerative capacity.
        Within general class of nervous system structural alteration, there 
    are various histological changes that can result after exposure to 
    neurotoxicants. For example, specific changes in nerve cell bodies 
    include chromatolysis, vacuolization, and cell death. Axons can undergo 
    swelling, degeneration, and atrophy, while myelin sheath changes 
    include folding, edematous splitting, and demyelination. Although 
    terminal degeneration does occur, it is not readily detectable by light 
    microscopy. Many of these changes are a result of complex effects at 
    specific subcellular organelles, such as the axonal swelling that 
    occurs as a result of neurofilament accumulation in acrylamide 
    toxicity. Other changes may be associated with regenerative or adaptive 
    processes that occur after neurotoxicant exposure.
    3.1.2.2. Neurophysiological Endpoints of Neurotoxicity
        Neurophysiological studies measure the electrical activity of the 
    nervous system. The term ``neurophysiology'' is often used synonymously 
    with ``electrophysiology'' (Dyer, 1987). Neurophysiological techniques 
    provide information on the integrity of defined portions of the nervous 
    system. Several neurophysiological procedures are available for 
    application to neurotoxicological studies. Examples are listed in Table 
    3. They range in scale from procedures that employ microelectrodes to 
    study the function of single nerve cells or restricted portions of 
    them, to procedures that employ macroelectrodes to perform simultaneous 
    recordings of the summed activity of many cells. Microelectrode 
    procedures typically are used to study mechanisms of action and are 
    frequently performed in vitro. Macroelectrode procedures are generally 
    used in studies to detect or characterize the potential neurotoxic 
    effects of agents of interest because of potential environmental 
    exposure. The present discussion concentrates on macroelectrode 
    neurophysiological procedures because it is more likely that they will 
    be the focus of decisions regarding critical effects in risk 
    assessment. All of the procedures described below for use in animals 
    also have been used in humans to determine chemically induced 
    alterations in neurophysiological function.
    
       Table 3.--Examples of Neurophysiological Measures of Neurotoxicity   
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
                                                            Representative  
             System/function               Procedure            agents      
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
    Retina..........................  Electroretinograph  Developmental     
                                       y (ERG).            lead.            
    Visual pathway..................  Flash-evoked        Carbon disulfide. 
                                       potential (FEP).                     
    Visual function.................  Pattern-evoked      Carbon disulfide. 
                                       potential (PEP)                      
                                       (pattern size and                    
                                       contrast).                           
    Auditory pathway................  Brain stem          Aminoglycoside,   
                                       auditory evoked     antibiotics,     
                                       potential (BAER)    toluene, styrene.
                                       (clicks).                            
    Auditory function...............  BAER (tones)......  Aminoglycoside,   
                                                           antibiotics,     
                                                           toluene, styrene.
    Somatosensory pathway...........  Somatosensory       Acrylamide, n-    
                                       provoked.           hexane.          
    Somatosensory function..........  Sensory-evoked      Acrylamide, n-    
                                       potential (SEP)     hexane.          
                                       (tactile).                           
    Spinocerebellar pathway.........  SEP recorded from   Acrylamide, n-    
                                       cerebellum.         hexane.          
    Mixed nerve.....................  Peripheral nerve    Triethyltin.      
                                       compound action                      
                                       potential (PNAP).                    
    Motor axons.....................  PNAP isolate motor  Triethyltin.      
                                       components.                          
    Sensory axons...................  PNAP isolate        Triethyltin.      
                                       sensory                              
                                       components.                          
    Neuromuscular...................  Electromyography    Dithiobiuret.     
                                       (EMG).                               
    General central nervous system/   Electroencephalogr  Toluene.          
     level of arousal.                 aphy (EEG).                          
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
    
        3.1.2.2.1. Nerve Conduction Studies. Nerve conduction studies, 
    generally performed on peripheral nerves, can be useful in 
    investigations of possible peripheral neuropathy. Most peripheral 
    nerves contain mixtures of individual sensory and motor nerve fibers, 
    which may or may not be differentially sensitive to neurotoxicants. It 
    is possible to distinguish sensory from motor effects in peripheral 
    nerve studies by measuring activity in sensory nerves or by measuring 
    the muscle response evoked by nerve stimulation to measure motor 
    effects. While a number of endpoints can be recorded, the most critical 
    variables are nerve conduction velocity, response amplitude, and 
    refractory period. It is important to recognize that damage to nerve 
    fibers may not be reflected in changes in these endpoints if the damage 
    is not sufficiently extensive. Thus, the interpretation of data from 
    such studies may be enhanced if evaluations such as nerve pathology 
    and/or other structural measures are also included.
        Nerve conduction measurements are influenced by a number of 
    factors, the most important of which is temperature. An adequate nerve 
    conduction study will either measure the temperature of the limb under 
    study and mathematically adjust the results according to well-
    established temperature factors or will control limb temperature within 
    narrow limits. Studies that measure peripheral nerve function without 
    regard for temperature are not adequate for risk assessment.
        In well-controlled studies, statistically significant decreases in 
    nerve conduction velocity are indicative of a neurotoxic effect. While 
    a decrease in nerve conduction velocity is indicative of demyelination, 
    it frequently occurs later in the course of axonal degradation because 
    normal conduction velocity may be maintained for some time in the face 
    of axonal degeneration. For this reason, a measurement of normal nerve 
    conduction velocity does not rule out peripheral axonal degeneration if 
    other signs of peripheral nerve dysfunction are present.
        Decreases in response amplitude reflect a loss of active nerve 
    fibers and may occur prior to decreases in conduction velocity in the 
    course of peripheral neuropathy. Hence, changes in response amplitude 
    may be more sensitive measurements of axonal degeneration than is 
    conduction velocity. Measurements of response amplitude, however, can 
    be more variable and require careful application of experimental 
    techniques, a larger sample size, and greater statistical power than 
    measurements of velocity to detect changes. The refractory period 
    refers to the time required after stimulation before a nerve can fire 
    again and reflects the functional status of nerve membrane ion 
    channels. Chemically induced changes in
    
    [[Page 26937]]
    
    refractory periods in a well-controlled study indicate a neurotoxic 
    effect.
        In summary, alterations in peripheral nerve response amplitude and 
    refractory period in studies that are well controlled for temperature 
    are indicative of a neurotoxic effect. Alterations in peripheral nerve 
    function are frequently associated with clinical signs such as 
    numbness, tingling, or burning sensations or with motor impairments 
    such as weakness. Examples of compounds that alter peripheral nerve 
    function in humans or experimental animals include acrylamide, carbon 
    disulfide, n-hexane, lead, and some organophosphates.
        3.1.2.2.2. Sensory, Motor, and Other Evoked Potentials. Evoked 
    potential studies are electrophysiological procedures that measure the 
    response elicited from a defined stimulus such as a tone, a light, or a 
    brief electrical pulse. Evoked potentials reflect the function of the 
    system under study, including visual, auditory, or somatosensory; 
    motor, involving motor nerves and innervated muscles; or other neural 
    pathways in the central or peripheral nervous system (Rebert, 1983; 
    Dyer, 1985; Mattsson and Albee, 1988; Mattsson et al., 1992; Boyes, 
    1992, 1993). Evoked potential studies should be interpreted with 
    respect to the known or presumed neural generators of the responses, 
    and their likely relationships with behavioral outcomes, when such 
    information is available. Such correlative information strengthens the 
    confidence in electrophysiological outcomes. In the absence of such 
    supportive information, the extent to which evoked potential studies 
    provide convincing evidence of neurotoxicity is a matter of 
    professional judgment on a case-by-case basis. Judgments should 
    consider the nature, magnitude, and duration of such effects, along 
    with other factors discussed elsewhere in this document.
        Data are in the form of a voltage record collected over time and 
    can be quantified in several ways. Commonly, the latency (time from 
    stimulus onset) and amplitude (voltage) of the positive and negative 
    voltage peaks are identified and measured. Alternative measurement 
    schemes may involve substitution of spectral phase or template shifts 
    for peak latency and spectral power, spectral amplitude, root-mean-
    square, or integrated area under the curve for peak amplitude. Latency 
    measurements are dependent on both the velocity of nerve conduction and 
    the time of synaptic transmission. Both of these factors depend on 
    temperature, as discussed in regard to nerve conduction, and similar 
    caveats apply for sensory evoked potential studies. In studies that are 
    well controlled for temperature, increases in latencies or related 
    measures can reflect deficits in nerve conduction, including 
    demyelination or delayed synaptic transmission, and are indicators of a 
    neurotoxic effect.
        Decreases in peak latencies, like increases in nerve conduction 
    velocity, are unusual, but the neural systems under study in sensory 
    evoked potentials are complex, and situations that might cause a peak 
    measurement to occur earlier are conceivable. Two such situations are a 
    reduced threshold for spatial or temporal summation of afferent neural 
    transmission and a selective loss of cells responding late in the peak, 
    thus making the measured peak occur earlier. Decreases in peak latency 
    should not be dismissed outright as experimental or statistical error, 
    but should be examined carefully and perhaps replicated to assess 
    possible neurotoxicity. A decrease in latency is not conclusive 
    evidence of a neurotoxic effect.
        Changes in peak amplitudes or equivalent measures reflect changes 
    in the magnitude of the neural population responsive to stimulation. 
    Both increases and decreases in amplitude are possible following 
    exposure to chemicals. Whether excitatory or inhibitory neural activity 
    is translated into a positive or negative deflection in the sensory 
    evoked potential is dependent on the physical orientation of the 
    electrode with respect to the tissue generating the response, which is 
    frequently unknown. Comparisons should be based on the absolute change 
    in amplitude. Therefore, either increases or decreases in amplitude may 
    be indicative of a neurotoxic effect.
        Within any given sensory system, the neural circuits that generate 
    various evoked potential peaks differ as a function of peak latency. In 
    general, early latency peaks reflect the transmission of afferent 
    sensory information. Changes in either the latency or amplitude of 
    these peaks are considered convincing evidence of a neurotoxic effect 
    that is likely to be reflected in deficits in sensory perception. The 
    later-latency peaks, in general, reflect not only the sensory input but 
    also the more nonspecific factors such as the behavioral state of the 
    subject, including such factors as arousal level, habituation, or 
    sensitization (Dyer, 1987). Thus, changes in later-latency evoked 
    potential peaks should be interpreted in light of the behavioral status 
    of the subject and would generally be considered evidence of a 
    neurotoxic effect.
        3.1.2.2.3. Seizures/Convulsions. Some neurotoxicants (e.g., 
    lindane, pyrethroids, trimethyltin, dichlorodiphenyltrichloroethane 
    [DDT]) produce observable convulsions. When convulsionlike behaviors 
    are observed, as described in the behavioral section on convulsions, 
    neurophysiological recordings can provide additional information to 
    help interpret the results. Recordings of brain electrical activity 
    that demonstrate seizurelike activity are indicative of a neurotoxic 
    effect.
        In addition to producing seizures directly, chemicals may also 
    alter the frequency, severity, duration, or threshold for eliciting 
    seizures through other means by a phenomenon known as ``kindling.'' 
    Such alterations can occur after acute exposure or after repeated 
    exposure to dose levels below the acute threshold. In experiments 
    demonstrating changes in sensitivity following repeated exposures to 
    the test compound, information regarding possible changes in the 
    pharmacokinetic distribution of the compound is required before the 
    seizure susceptibility changes can be interpreted as evidence of 
    neurotoxicity. Increases in susceptibility to seizures are considered 
    adverse.
        3.1.2.2.4. Electroencephalography (EEG). EEG analysis is used 
    widely in clinical settings for the diagnosis of neurological 
    disorders, and less often for the detection of subtle toxicant-induced 
    dysfunction (WHO, 1986; Eccles, 1988). The basis for using EEG in 
    either setting is the relationship between specific patterns of EEG 
    waveforms and specific behavioral states. Because states of alertness 
    and stages of sleep are associated with distinct patterns of electrical 
    activity in the brain, it is generally thought that arousal level can 
    be evaluated by monitoring the EEG. Dissociation of EEG activity and 
    behavior can, however, occur after exposure to certain chemicals. 
    Normal patterns of transition between sleep stages or between sleeping 
    and waking states are known to remain disturbed for prolonged periods 
    of time after exposure to some chemicals. Changes in the pattern of the 
    EEG can be elicited by anesthetic drugs and stimuli producing arousal 
    (e.g., lights, sounds). In studies with toxicants, changes in EEG 
    pattern can sometimes precede alterations in other objective signs of 
    neurotoxicity (Dyer, 1987).
        EEG studies should be done under highly controlled conditions, and 
    the data should be considered on a case-by-case basis. Chemically 
    induced seizure activity detected in the EEG pattern is evidence of a 
    neurotoxic effect.
    
    [[Page 26938]]
    
    3.1.2.3. Neurochemical Endpoints of Neurotoxicity
        Many different neurochemical endpoints have been measured in 
    neurotoxicological studies, and some have proven useful in advancing 
    the understanding of mechanisms of action of neurotoxic chemicals 
    (Bondy, 1986; Mailman, 1987; Morell and Mailman, 1987; Costa, 1988; 
    Silbergeld, 1993). Normal functioning of the nervous system depends on 
    the synthesis and release of specific neurotransmitters and activation 
    of their receptors at specific presynaptic and postsynaptic sites. 
    Chemicals can interfere with the ionic balance of a neuron, act as a 
    cytotoxicant after transport into a nerve terminal, block reuptake of 
    neurotransmitters and their precursors, act as a metabolic poison, 
    overstimulate receptors, block transmitter release, and inhibit 
    transmitter synthetic or catabolic enzymes. Table 4 lists several 
    chemicals that produce neurotoxic effects at the neurochemical level 
    (Bondy, 1986; Mailman, 1987; Morell and Mailman, 1987; Costa, 1988).
    
    Table 4.--Examples of Neurotoxicants With Known Neurochemical Mechanisms
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
                 Site of action                          Examples           
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
    Neurotoxicants acting on ionic balance:                                 
      Inhibit sodium entry.................  Tetrodotoxin.                  
      Block closing of sodium channel......  p,p'-DDT, pyrethroids.         
      Increase permeability to sodium......  Batrachotoxin.                 
      Increase intracellular calcium.......  Chlorodecone.                  
    Synaptic neurotoxicants................  MPTP.                          
    Uptake blockers........................  Hemicholinium.                 
    Metabolic poisons......................  Cyanide.                       
    Hyperactivation of receptors...........  Domoic acid.                   
    Blocks transmitter release.............  Botulinum toxin.               
    Inhibition of transmitter degradation..  Pesticides of the              
                                              organophosphate and carbamate 
                                              classes.                      
    Blocks axonal transport................  Acrylamide.                    
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
    
        As stated previously, any neurochemical change is potentially 
    neurotoxic. Persistent or irreversible chemically induced neurochemical 
    changes are indicative of neurotoxicity. Because the ultimate 
    functional significance of some biochemical changes is not known at 
    this time, neurochemical studies should be interpreted with reference 
    to the presumed neurotoxic consequence(s) of the neurochemical changes. 
    For example, many neuroactive agents can increase or decrease 
    neurotransmitter levels, but such changes are not indicative of a 
    neurotoxic effect. If, however, these neurochemical changes may be 
    expected to have neurophysiological, neuropathological, or 
    neurobehavioral correlates, then the neurochemical changes could be 
    classified as neurotoxic effects.
        Some neurotoxicants, such as the organophosphate and carbamate 
    pesticides, are known to inhibit the activity of a specific enzyme, 
    acetylcholinesterase (for a review see Costa, 1988), which hydrolyzes 
    the neurotransmitter acetylcholine. Inhibition of the enzyme in either 
    the central or peripheral nervous system prolongs the action of the 
    acetylcholine at the neuron's synaptic receptors and is thought to be 
    responsible for the range of effects these chemicals produce, although 
    it is possible that these compounds have other modes of action 
    (Eldefrawi et al., 1992; Greenfield et al., 1984; Small, 1990).
        There is agreement that objective clinical measures of cholinergic 
    overstimulation (e.g., salivation, sweating, muscle weakness, tremor, 
    blurred vision) can be used to evaluate dose-response and dose-effect 
    relationships and define the presence and absence of effects. A given 
    depression in peripheral and central cholinesterase activity may or may 
    not be accompanied by clinical manifestations. A depression in RBC and/
    or plasma cholinesterase activity may or may not be accompanied by 
    clinical manifestations. It should be noted, however, that reduction in 
    cholinesterase activity, even if the anticholinesterase exposure is not 
    severe enough to precipitate clinical signs or symptoms, may impair the 
    organism's ability to adapt to additional exposures to 
    anticholinesterase compounds. Inhibition of RBC and/or plasma 
    cholinesterase activity is a biomarker of exposure, as well as a 
    reflection of cholinesterase inhibition in other peripheral tissues 
    (e.g., neuromuscular junction, peripheral nerve, or ganglia) (Maxwell 
    et al., 1987; Nagymajtenyi et al., 1988; Padilla et al., 1994), thereby 
    contributing to the overall hazard identification of cholinesterase-
    inhibiting compounds.
        The risk assessor should also be aware that tolerance to the 
    cholinergic overstimulation may be observed following repeated exposure 
    to cholinesterase-inhibiting chemicals. It has been reported, however, 
    that although tolerance can develop to some effects of cholinesterase 
    inhibition, the cellular mechanisms responsible for the development of 
    tolerance may also lead to the development of other effects, i.e., 
    cognitive dysfunction, not present at the time of initial exposure 
    (Bushnell et al., 1991). These adaptive biochemical changes in the 
    tolerant animal may render it supersensitive to subsequent exposure to 
    cholinergically active compounds (Pope et al., 1992).
        In general, the risk assessor should understand that assessment of 
    cholinesterase-inhibiting chemicals should be done on a case-by-case 
    basis using a weight-of evidence approach in which all of the available 
    data (e.g., brain, blood, and other tissue cholinesterase activity, as 
    well as the presence or absence of clinical signs) is considered in the 
    evaluation. Generally, the toxic effects of anticholinesterase 
    compounds are viewed as reversible, but there is human and experimental 
    animal evidence indicating that there may be residual, if not 
    permanent, effects of exposure to these compounds (Steenland et al., 
    1994; Tandon et al., 1994; Stephens et al., 1995).
        A subset of organophosphate agents also produces organophosphate-
    induced delayed neuropathy (OPIDN) after acute or repeated exposure. 
    Inhibition and aging of neurotoxic esterase (or neuropathy enzymes) are 
    associated with agents that produce OPIDN (Johnson, 1990; Richardson, 
    1995). The conclusion that a chemical may produce OPIDN should be based 
    on at least two of three factors: (1) Evidence of a clinical syndrome, 
    (2) pathological lesions, and (3) neurotoxic esterase (NTE) inhibition. 
    NTE inhibition is necessary, but not sufficient, evidence
    
    [[Page 26939]]
    
    of the potential to produce OPIDN when there is at least 55%-70% 
    inhibition after acute exposure (Ehrich et al., 1995) and at least 45% 
    inhibition following repeated exposure.
        Chemically induced injury to the central nervous system may be 
    accompanied by hypertrophy of astrocytes. In some cases, these 
    astrocytic changes can be seen light microscopically with 
    immunohistochemical stains for glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP), 
    the major intermediate filament protein in astrocytes. In addition, 
    GFAP can be quantified by an immunoassay, which has been proposed as a 
    marker of astrocyte reactivity (O'Callaghan, 1988). Immunohistochemical 
    stains have the advantage of better localization of GFAP increases, 
    whereas immunoassay evaluations are superior at detecting and 
    quantifying changes in GFAP levels and establishing dose-response 
    relationships. The ability to detect and quantify changes in GFAP by 
    immunoassay is improved by dissecting and analyzing multiple brain 
    regions. The interpretation of a chemical-induced change in GFAP is 
    facilitated by corroborative data from the neuropathology or 
    neuroanatomy evaluation. A number of chemicals known to injure the 
    central nervous system, including trimethyltin, methylmercury, cadmium, 
    3-acetylpyridine, and methylphenyltetrahydropyridine (MPTP), have been 
    shown to increase levels of GFAP. Measures of GFAP are now included as 
    an optional test in the Neurotoxicity Screening Battery (U.S. EPA, 
    1991a).
        Increases in GFAP above control levels may be seen at dosages below 
    those necessary to produce damage seen by standard microscopic or 
    histopathological techniques. Because increases in GFAP reflect an 
    astrocyte response in adults, treatment-related increases in GFAP are 
    considered to be evidence that a neurotoxic effect has occurred. There 
    is less agreement as to how to interpret decreases in GFAP relative to 
    an appropriate control group. The absence of a change in GFAP following 
    exposure does not mean that the chemical is devoid of neurotoxic 
    potential. Known neurotoxicants such as cholinesterase-inhibiting 
    pesticides, for example, would not be expected to increase brain levels 
    of GFAP. Interpretation of GFAP changes prior to weaning may be 
    confounded by the possibility that chemically induced increases in GFAP 
    could be masked by changes in the concentration of this protein 
    associated with maturation of the central nervous system, and these 
    data may be difficult to interpret.
    3.1.2.4. Behavioral Endpoints of Neurotoxicity
        Behavior reflects the integration of the various functional 
    components of the nervous system. Changes in behavior can arise from a 
    direct effect of a toxicant on the nervous system, or indirectly from 
    its effects on other physiological systems. Understanding the 
    interrelationship between systemic toxicity and behavioral changes 
    (e.g., the relationship between liver damage and motor activity) is 
    extremely important. The presence of systemic toxicity may complicate, 
    but does not preclude, interpretation of behavioral changes as evidence 
    of neurotoxicity. In addition, a number of behaviors (e.g., schedule-
    controlled behavior) may require a motivational component for 
    successful completion of the task. In such cases, experimental 
    paradigms designed to assess the motivation of an animal during 
    behavior might be necessary to interpret the meaning of some chemical-
    induced changes in behavior.
        EPA's testing guidelines developed for the Toxic Substances Control 
    Act and the Federal Insecticide, Fungicide and Rodenticide Act describe 
    the use of functional observational batteries (FOB), motor activity, 
    and schedule-controlled behavior for assessing neurotoxic potential 
    (U.S. EPA, 1991a). Examples of measures obtained in a typical FOB are 
    presented in Table 5. There are many other measures of behavior, 
    including specialized tests of motor and sensory function and of 
    learning and memory (Tilson, 1987; Anger, 1984).
    
          Table 5.--Examples of Measures in a Representative Functional     
                              Observational Battery                         
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
        Home cage and open field         Manipulative        Physiological  
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
    Arousal.........................  Approach response.  Body temperature. 
    Autonomic signs.................  Click response....  Body weight.      
    Convulsions, tremors............  Foot splay........                    
    Gait............................  Grip strength.....  ..................
    Mobility........................  Righting reflex...  ..................
    Posture.........................  Tail pinch          ..................
                                       response.                            
    Rearing.                                                                
    Stereotypy.                                                             
    Touch response.                   ..................  ..................
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
    
    
          Table 6.--Examples of Specialized Behavioral Tests To Measure     
                                  Neurotoxicity                             
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
                                                            Representative  
                Function                   Procedure            agents      
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
                                 Motor Function                             
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
    Weakness........................  Grip strength,      n-Hexane, methyl. 
                                       swimming           n-Butylketone,    
                                       endurance,          carbaryl.        
                                       suspension rod,                      
                                       discriminative                       
                                       motor function.                      
    Incoordination..................  Rotorod, gait       3-Acetylpyridine, 
                                       assessments,        ethanol.         
                                       righting reflex.                     
    Tremor..........................  Rating scale,       Chlordecone, Type 
                                       spectral analysis.  I.               
                                                          pyrethroids, DDT. 
    Myoclonic spasms................  Rating scale......  DDT, Type II      
                                                           pyrethroids.     
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
                                Sensory Function                            
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
    Auditory........................  Discrimination      Toluene,          
                                       conditioning.       trimethyltin.    
                                      Reflex              ..................
                                       modification.                        
    Visual..........................  Discrimination      Methylmercury.    
                                       conditioning.                        
    Somatosensory...................  Discrimination      Acrylamide.       
                                       conditioning.                        
    Pain sensitivity................  Discrimination      Parathion.        
                                       conditioning.                        
    Olfactory.......................  Discrimination      3-Methylindole,   
                                       conditioning.       methylbromide.   
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
                                Cognitive Function                          
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
    Habituation.....................  Startle reflex....  Diisopropylfluorop
                                                           hosphate.        
                                                          Pre/neonatal      
                                                           methylmercury.   
    
    [[Page 26940]]
    
                                                                            
    Classical conditioning..........  Nictitating         Aluminum.         
                                       membrane.                            
                                      Conditioned flavor  Carbaryl.         
                                      aversion..........  Trimethyltin,     
                                                           IDPN.            
                                      Passive avoidance.  Neonatal          
                                                           trimethyltin.    
                                      Olfactory           ..................
                                       conditioning.                        
    Instrumental conditioning.......  One-way avoidance.  Chlordecone.      
                                      Two-way avoidance.  Pre/neonatal lead.
                                      Y-maze avoidance..  Hypervitaminosis  
                                                           A.               
                                      Biel water maze...  Styrene.          
                                      Morris water maze.  DFP.              
                                      Radial arm maze...  Trimethyltin.     
                                      Delayed matching    DFP.              
                                       to sample.                           
                                      Repeated            Carbaryl.         
                                       acquisition.                         
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
    
        At the present time, there is no clear consensus concerning the use 
    of specific behavioral tests to assess chemical-induced sensory, motor, 
    or cognitive dysfunction in animal models. The risk assessor should 
    also know that the literature is clear that a number of other behaviors 
    besides those listed in Tables 1, 5, and 6 could be affected by 
    chemical exposure. For example, alterations in food and water intake, 
    reproduction, sleep, temperature regulation, and circadian rhythmicity 
    are controlled by specific regions of the brain, and chemical-induced 
    alterations in these behaviors could be indicative of neurotoxicity. It 
    is reasonable to assume that an NOAEL or LOAEL could be based on one or 
    more of these endpoints.
        The following sections describe, in general, behavioral tests and 
    their uses and offer guidance on interpreting data.
        3.1.2.4.1. Functional Observational Battery (FOB). An FOB is 
    designed to detect and quantify major overt behavioral, physiological, 
    and neurological signs (Gad, 1982; O'Donoghue, 1989; Moser, 1989). A 
    number of batteries have been developed, each consisting of tests 
    generally intended to evaluate various aspects of sensorimotor function 
    (Tilson and Moser, 1992). Many FOB tests are essentially clinical 
    neurological examinations that rate the presence or absence, and in 
    many cases the severity, of specific neurological signs. Some FOBs in 
    animals are similar to clinical neurological examinations used with 
    human patients. Most FOBs have several components or tests. A typical 
    FOB is summarized in Table 5 and evaluates several functional domains, 
    including neuromuscular (i.e., weakness, incoordination, gait, and 
    tremor), sensory (i.e., audition, vision, and somatosensory), and 
    autonomic (i.e., pupil response and salivation) function.
        The relevance of statistically significant test results from an FOB 
    is judged according to the number of signs affected, the dose(s) at 
    which effects are observed, and the nature, severity, and persistence 
    of the effects and their incidence in relation to control animals. In 
    general, if only a few unrelated measures in the FOB are affected, or 
    the effects are unrelated to dose, the results may not be considered 
    evidence of a neurotoxic effect. If several neurological signs are 
    affected, but only at the high dose and in conjunction with other overt 
    signs of toxicity, including systemic toxicity, large decreases in body 
    weight, decreases in body temperature, or debilitation, there is less 
    persuasive evidence of a direct neurotoxic effect. In cases where 
    several related measures in a battery of tests are affected and the 
    effects appear to be dose dependent, the data are considered to be 
    evidence of a neurotoxic effect, especially in the absence of systemic 
    toxicity. The risk assessor should be aware of the potential for a 
    number of false positive statistical findings in these studies because 
    of the large number of endpoints customarily included in the FOB.
        FOB data can be grouped into one or more of several neurobiological 
    domains, including neuromuscular (i.e., weakness, incoordination, 
    abnormal movements, gait), sensory (i.e., auditory, visual, 
    somatosensory), and autonomic functions (Tilson and Moser, 1992). This 
    statistical technique may be useful when separating changes that occur 
    on the basis of chance or in conjunction with systemic toxicity from 
    those treatment-related changes indicative of neurotoxic effects. In 
    the case of the developing organism, chemicals may alter the maturation 
    or appearance of sensorimotor reflexes. Significant alterations in or 
    delay of such reflexes is evidence of a neurotoxic effect.
        Examples of chemicals that affect neuromuscular function are 3-
    acetylpyridine, acrylamide, and triethyltin. Organophosphate and 
    carbamate insecticides produce autonomic dysfunction, while 
    organochlorine and pyrethroid insecticides increase sensorimotor 
    sensitivity, produce tremors and, in some cases, cause seizures and 
    convulsions (Spencer and Schaumburg, 1980).
        3.1.2.4.2. Motor Activity. Motor activity represents a broad class 
    of behaviors involving coordinated participation of sensory, motor, and 
    integrative processes. Assessment of motor activity is noninvasive and 
    has been used to evaluate the effects of acute and repeated exposure to 
    neurotoxicants (MacPhail et al., 1989). An organism's level of activity 
    can, however, be affected by many different types of environmental 
    agents, including non-neurotoxic agents. Motor activity measurements 
    also have been used in humans to evaluate disease states, including 
    disorders of the nervous system (Goldstein and Stein, 1985).
        Motor activity is usually quantified as the frequency of movements 
    over a period of time. The total counts generated during a test period 
    will depend on the recording mechanism and the size and configuration 
    of the testing apparatus. Effects of agents on motor activity can be 
    expressed as absolute activity counts or as a percentage of control 
    values. In some cases, a transformation (e.g., square root) may be used 
    to achieve a normal distribution of the data. In these cases, the 
    transformed data and not raw data should be used for risk assessment 
    purposes. The frequency of motor activity within a session usually 
    decreases and is reported as the average number of counts occurring in 
    each successive block of time. The EPA's
    
    [[Page 26941]]
    
    Office of Prevention, Pesticides and Toxic Substances guidelines (U.S. 
    EPA, 1991a), for example, call for test sessions of sufficient duration 
    to allow motor activity to approach steady-state levels during the last 
    20 percent of the session for control animals. A sum of the counts in 
    each epoch will add up to the total number of counts per session.
        Motor activity can be altered by a number of experimental factors, 
    including neurotoxic chemicals. Decreases in activity could occur 
    following high doses of non-neurotoxic agents (Kotsonis and Klaassen, 
    1977; Landauer et al., 1984). Examples of neurotoxic agents that 
    decrease motor activity include many pesticides (e.g., carbamates, 
    chlorinated hydrocarbons, organophosphates, and pyrethroids), heavy 
    metals (lead, tin, and mercury), and other agents (3-acetylpyridine, 
    acrylamide, and 2,4-dithiobiuret). Some neurotoxicants (e.g., toluene, 
    xylene, triadimefon) produce transient increases in activity by 
    presumably stimulating neurotransmitter release, while others (e.g., 
    trimethyltin) produce persistent increases in motor activity by 
    destroying specific regions of the brain (e.g., hippocampus).
        Following developmental exposures, neurotoxic effects are often 
    observed as a change in the ontogenetic profile or maturation of motor 
    activity patterns. Frequently, developmental exposure to neurotoxic 
    agents will produce an increase in motor activity that persists into 
    adulthood or that results in changes in other behaviors. This is 
    evidence of a neurotoxic effect. Like other organ systems, the nervous 
    system may be differentially sensitive to toxicants in groups such as 
    the young. For example, toxicants introduced to the developing nervous 
    system may kill stem cells and thus cause profound effects on adult 
    structure and function. Moreover, toxicants may have greater access to 
    the developing nervous system before the blood-brain barrier is 
    completely formed or before metabolic detoxifying systems are 
    functional.
        Motor activity measurements are typically used with other tests 
    (e.g., FOB) to help detect neurotoxic effects. Agent-induced changes in 
    motor activity associated with other overt signs of toxicity (e.g., 
    loss of body weight, systemic toxicity) or occurring in non-dose-
    related fashion are of less concern than changes that are dose 
    dependent, are related to structural or other functional changes in the 
    nervous system, or occur in the absence of life-threatening toxicity.
        13.1.2.4.3. Schedule-Controlled Operant Behavior. Schedule-
    controlled operant behavior (SCOB) involves the maintenance of behavior 
    (e.g., performance of a lever-press or key-peck response) by 
    reinforcement. Different rates and patterns of responding are 
    controlled by the relationship between response and subsequent 
    reinforcement. SCOB provides a measure of performance of a learned 
    behavior (e.g., lever press or key peck) and involves training and 
    motivational variables that should be considered in evaluating the 
    data. Agents may interact with sensory processing, motor output, 
    motivational variables (i.e., related to reinforcement), training 
    history, and baseline characteristics (Rice, 1988; Cory-Slechta, 1989). 
    Qualitatively, rates and patterns of SCOB display cross-species 
    generality, but the quantitative measures of rate and pattern of 
    performance can vary within and between species.
        In laboratory animals, SCOB has been used to study a wide range of 
    neurotoxicants, including methylmercury, many pesticides, organic and 
    inorganic lead, triethyltin, and trimethyltin (MacPhail, 1985; Tilson, 
    1987; Rice, 1988). The primary SCOB endpoints for evaluation are 
    response rate and the temporal pattern of responding. These endpoints 
    may vary as a function of the contingency between responding and 
    reinforcement presentation (i.e., schedule of reinforcement). Schedules 
    of reinforcement that have been used in toxicology studies include 
    fixed ratio and fixed interval schedules. Fixed ratio schedules 
    engender high rates of responding and a characteristic pause after 
    delivery of each reinforcement. Fixed interval schedules engender a 
    relatively low rate of responding during the initial portion of the 
    interval and progressively higher rates near the end of the interval. 
    For some schedules of reinforcement, the temporal pattern of responding 
    may play a more important role in defining the performance 
    characteristics than the rate of responding. For other schedules, the 
    reverse may be true. For example, the temporal pattern of responding 
    may be more important than rate of responding for defining performance 
    on a fixed interval schedule. For a fixed ratio schedule, more 
    importance might be placed on the rate of responding than on the post-
    reinforcement pause.
        The overall qualitative patterns are important properties of the 
    behavior. Substantial qualitative changes in operant performance, such 
    as elimination of characteristic response patterns, can be evidence of 
    an adverse effect. Most chemicals, however, can disrupt operant 
    behavior at some dose, and such adverse effects may be due either to 
    neurotoxic or non-neurotoxic mechanisms. Unlike large qualitative 
    changes in operant performance, small quantitative changes are not 
    adverse. Some changes may actually represent an improvement, e.g., an 
    increase in the index of curvature with a decrease in fixed interval 
    rate of responding. Assessing the toxicological importance of these 
    effects requires considerable professional judgment and evaluation of 
    converging evidence from other types of toxicological endpoints. While 
    most chemicals decrease the efficiency of responding at some dose, some 
    agents may increase response efficiency on schedules requiring high 
    response rates because of a stimulant effect or an increase in central 
    nervous system excitability. Agent-induced changes in responding 
    between reinforcements (i.e., the temporal pattern of responding) may 
    occur independently of changes in the overall rate of responding. 
    Chemicals may also affect the reaction time to respond following 
    presentation of a stimulus. Agent-induced changes in response rate or 
    temporal patterning associated with other overt signs of toxicity 
    (e.g., body weight loss, systemic toxicity, or occurring in a non-dose-
    related fashion) are of less concern than changes that are dose 
    dependent, related to structural or other functional changes in the 
    nervous system, or occur in the absence of life-threatening toxicity.
    3.1.2.4.4. Convulsions. Observable convulsions in animals are 
    indicative of an adverse effect. These events can reflect central 
    nervous system activity comparable to that of epilepsy in humans and 
    could be defined as neurotoxicity. Occasionally, other toxic actions 
    of compounds, such as direct effects on muscle, might mimic some 
    convulsionlike behaviors. In some cases, convulsions or 
    convulsionlike behaviors may be observed in animals that are 
    otherwise severely compromised, moribund, or near death. In such 
    cases, convulsions might reflect an indirect effect of systemic 
    toxicity and are less clearly indicative of neurotoxicity. As 
    discussed in the section on neurophysiological measures, electrical 
    recordings of brain activity could be used to determine specificity 
    of effects on the nervous system.
        3.1.2.4.5. Specialized Tests for Neurotoxicity. Several procedures 
    have been developed to measure agent-induced changes in specific 
    neurobehavioral functions such as motor, sensory, or cognitive function 
    (Tilson, 1987; Cory-Slechta, 1989). Table 6 lists several behavioral 
    tests, the neurobehavioral functions they were designed to assess, and 
    agents known to affect the response. Many of these tests in animals 
    have been designed to assess neural functions in humans using similar 
    testing procedures.
        A statistically or biologically significant chemically induced 
    change
    
    [[Page 26942]]
    
    in any measure in Table 6 may be evidence of an adverse effect. 
    However, judgments of neurotoxicity may involve not only the analysis 
    of changes seen but the structure and class of the chemical and other 
    available neurochemical, neurophysiological, and neuropathological 
    evidence. In general, behavioral changes seen across broader dose 
    ranges indicate more specific actions on the systems underlying those 
    changes, i.e., the nervous system. Changes that are not dose dependent 
    or that are confounded with body weight changes and/or other systemic 
    toxicity may be more difficult to interpret as neurotoxic effects.
        3.1.2.4.5.1. Motor Function. Neurotoxicants commonly affect motor 
    function. These effects can be categorized generally into (1) weakness 
    or decreased strength, (2) tremor, (3) incoordination, and (4) spasms, 
    myoclonia, or abnormal motor movements (Tilson, 1987; Cory-Slechta, 
    1989). Specialized tests used to assess strength include measures of 
    grip strength, swimming endurance, suspension from a hanging rod, and 
    discriminative motor function. Rotorod and gait assessments are used to 
    measure coordination, while rating scales and spectral analysis 
    techniques can be used to quantify tremor and other abnormal movements.
        3.1.2.4.5.2. Sensory Function. Gross perturbations of sensory 
    function can be observed in simple neurological assessments such as the 
    hot plate or tail flick test. However, these tests may not be 
    sufficiently sensitive to detect subtle sensory changes. Psychophysical 
    procedures that study the relationship between a physical dimension 
    (e.g., intensity, frequency) of a stimulus and behavior may be 
    necessary to quantify agent-induced alterations in sensory function. 
    Examples of psychophysical procedures include discriminated 
    conditioning and startle reflex modification.
        3.1.2.4.5.3. Cognitive Function. Alterations in learning and memory 
    in experimental animals should be inferred from changes in behavior 
    following exposure when compared with that seen prior to exposure or 
    with a nonexposed control group. Learning is defined as a relatively 
    lasting change in behavior due to experience, and memory is defined as 
    the persistence of a learned behavior over time. Table 6 lists several 
    examples of learning and memory tests and representative neurotoxicants 
    known to affect these tests. Measurement of changes in learning and 
    memory should be separated from other changes in behavior that do not 
    involve cognitive or associative processes (i.e., motor function, 
    sensory capabilities, motivational factors). In addition, any apparent 
    toxicant-induced change in learning or memory should ideally be 
    demonstrated over a range of stimulus and response conditions and 
    testing conditions. In developmental exposures, it should be shown that 
    the animals have matured enough to perform the specified task. 
    Developmental neurotoxicants can accelerate or delay the ability to 
    learn a response or may interfere with cognitive function at the time 
    of testing. Older animals frequently perform poorly on some types of 
    tests, and it should be demonstrated that control animals in this 
    population are capable of performing the procedure. Neurotoxicants 
    might accelerate age-related dysfunction or alter motivational 
    variables that are important for learning to occur. Further, it is not 
    the case that a decrease in responding on a learning task is adverse 
    while an increase in performance on a learning task is not. It is well 
    known that lesions in certain regions of the brain can facilitate the 
    acquisition of certain types of behaviors by removing preexisting 
    response tendencies (e.g., inhibitory responses due to stress) that 
    moderate the rate of learning under normal circumstances.
        Apparent improvement in performance is not either adverse or 
    beneficial until demonstrated to be so by converging evidence with a 
    variety of experimental methods. Examples of procedures to assess 
    cognitive function include simple habituation, classical conditioning, 
    and operant (or instrumental) conditioning, including tests for spatial 
    learning and memory.
        3.1.2.4.5.4. Developmental Neurotoxicity. Although the previous 
    discussion of various neurotoxicity endpoints and tests applies to 
    studies in which developmental exposures are used, there are particular 
    issues of importance in the evaluation of developmental neurotoxicity 
    studies. This section underscores the importance of detecting 
    neurotoxic effects following developmental exposure because an NRC 
    (1993) report has indicated that infants and children may be 
    differentially sensitive to environmental chemicals such as pesticides. 
    Exposure to chemicals during development can result in a spectrum of 
    effects, including death, structural abnormalities, altered growth, and 
    functional deficits (U.S. EPA, 1991b). A number of agents have been 
    shown to cause developmental neurotoxicity when exposure occurred 
    during the period between conception and sexual maturity (e.g., Riley 
    and Vorhees, 1986; Vorhees, 1987).
        Table 7 lists several examples of agents known to produce 
    developmental neurotoxicity in experimental animals. Animal models of 
    developmental neurotoxicity have been shown to be sensitive to several 
    environmental agents known to produce developmental neurotoxicity in 
    humans, including lead, ethanol, x-irradiation, methylmercury, and 
    polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) (Kimmel et al., 1990; Needleman, 1990; 
    Jacobson et al., 1985; Needleman, 1986). In many of these cases, 
    functional deficits are observed at dose levels below those at which 
    other indicators of developmental toxicity are evident or at minimally 
    toxic doses in adults. Such effects may be transient, but generally are 
    considered adverse. Developmental exposure to a chemical could result 
    in transient or reversible effects observed during early development 
    that could reemerge as the individual ages (Barone et al., 1995).
    
      Table 7.--Examples of Compounds or Treatments Producing Developmental 
                                  Neurotoxicity                             
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
                                                                            
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
    Alcohols..................................  Methanol, ethanol.          
    Antimitotics..............................  X-radiation, azacytidine.   
    Insecticides..............................  DDT, chlordecone.           
    Metals....................................  Lead, methylmercury,        
                                                 cadmium.                   
    Polyhalogenated hydrocarbons..............  PCBs, PBBs.                 
    ------------------------------------------------------------------------
    
        Testing for developmental neurotoxicity has not been required 
    routinely by regulatory agencies in the United States, but is required 
    by EPA when other information indicates the potential for developmental 
    neurotoxicity (U.S. EPA, 1986, 1988a, 1988b, 1989, 1991a, 1991b). 
    Useful data for decision making may be derived from well-conducted 
    adult neurotoxicity studies, standard developmental toxicity studies, 
    and multigeneration studies, although the dose levels used in the 
    latter may be lower than those in studies with shorter term exposure.
        Important design issues to be evaluated for developmental 
    neurotoxicity studies are similar to those for standard developmental 
    toxicity studies (e.g., a dose-response approach with the highest dose 
    producing minimal overt maternal or perinatal toxicity, with number of 
    litters large enough for adequate statistical power, with randomization 
    of animals to dose groups and test groups, with litter generally 
    considered as the statistical unit). In addition, the use of a 
    replicate study design provides added confidence in the interpretation 
    of data. A pharmacological/physiological challenge may also be valuable 
    in
    
    [[Page 26943]]
    
    evaluating neurological function and ``unmasking'' effects not 
    otherwise detectable. For example, a challenge with a psychomotor 
    stimulant such as d-amphetamine may unmask latent developmental 
    neurotoxicity (Hughes and Sparber, 1978; Adams and Buelke-Sam, 1981; 
    Buelke-Sam et al., 1985).
        Direct extrapolation of developmental neurotoxicity to humans is 
    limited in the same way as for other endpoints of toxicity, i.e., by 
    the lack of knowledge about underlying toxicological mechanisms and 
    their significance (U.S. EPA, 1991b). However, comparisons of human and 
    animal data for several agents known to cause developmental 
    neurotoxicity in humans showed many similarities in effects (Kimmel et 
    al., 1990). As evidenced primarily by observations in laboratory 
    animals, comparisons at the level of functional category (sensory, 
    motivational, cognitive, motor function, and social behavior) showed 
    close agreement across species for the agents evaluated, even though 
    the specific endpoints used to assess these functions varied 
    considerably across species (Stanton and Spear, 1990). Thus, it can be 
    assumed that developmental neurotoxicity effects in animal studies 
    indicate the potential for altered neurobehavioral development in 
    humans, although the specific types of developmental effects seen in 
    experimental animal studies will not be the same as those that may be 
    produced in humans. Therefore, when data suggesting adverse effects in 
    developmental neurotoxicity studies are encountered for particular 
    agents, they should be considered in the risk assessment process.
        Functional tests with a moderate degree of background variability 
    (e.g., a coefficient of variability of 20% or less) may be more 
    sensitive to the effects of an agent on behavioral endpoints than are 
    tests with low variability that may be impossible to disrupt without 
    using life-threatening doses. A battery of functional tests, in 
    contrast to a single test, is usually needed to evaluate the full 
    complement of nervous system functions in an animal. Likewise, a series 
    of tests conducted in animals in several age groups may provide more 
    information about maturational changes and their persistence than tests 
    conducted at a single age.
        It is a well-established principle that there are critical 
    developmental periods for the disruption of functional competence, 
    which include both the prenatal and postnatal periods to the time of 
    sexual maturation, and the effect of a toxicant is likely to vary 
    depending on the time and degree of exposure (Rodier, 1978, 1990). It 
    is also important to consider the data from studies in which postnatal 
    exposure is included, as there may be an interaction of the agent with 
    maternal behavior, milk composition, or pup suckling behavior, as well 
    as possible direct exposure of pups via dosed food or water (Kimmel et 
    al., 1992).
        Agents that produce developmental neurotoxicity at a dose that is 
    not toxic to the maternal animal are of special concern. However, 
    adverse developmental effects are often produced at doses that cause 
    mild maternal toxicity (e.g., 10%-20% reduction in weight gain during 
    gestation and lactation). At doses causing moderate maternal toxicity 
    (i.e., 20% or more reduction in weight gain during gestation and 
    lactation), interpretation of developmental effects may be confounded. 
    Current information is inadequate to assume that developmental effects 
    at doses causing minimal maternal toxicity result only from maternal 
    toxicity; rather, it may be that the mother and developing organism are 
    equally sensitive to that dose level. Moreover, whether developmental 
    effects are secondary to maternal toxicity or not, the maternal effects 
    may be reversible while the effects on the offspring may be permanent. 
    These are important considerations for agents to which humans may be 
    exposed at minimally toxic levels either voluntarily or involuntarily, 
    because several agents (e.g., alcohol) are known to produce adverse 
    developmental effects at minimally toxic doses in adult humans (Coles 
    et al., 1991).
        Although interpretation of developmental neurotoxicity data may be 
    limited, it is clear that functional effects should be evaluated in 
    light of other toxicity data, including other forms of developmental 
    toxicity (e.g., structural abnormalities, perinatal death, and growth 
    retardation). For example, alterations in motor performance may be due 
    to a skeletal malformation rather than nervous system change. Changes 
    in learning tasks that require a visual cue might be influenced by 
    structural abnormalities in the eye. The level of confidence that an 
    agent produces an adverse effect may be as important as the type of 
    change seen, and confidence may be increased by such factors as 
    reproducibility of the effect, either in another study of the same 
    function or by convergence of data from tests that purport to measure 
    similar functions. A dose-response relationship is an extremely 
    important measure of a chemical's effect; in the case of developmental 
    neurotoxicity both monotonic and biphasic dose-response curves are 
    likely, depending on the function being tested. The EPA Guidelines for 
    Developmental Toxicity Risk Assessment (U.S. EPA, 1991b) may be 
    consulted for more information on interpreting developmental toxicity 
    studies. The endpoints frequently used to assess developmental 
    neurotoxicity in exposed children have been reviewed by Winneke (1995).
    3.1.3. Other Considerations
    3.1.3.1. Pharmacokinetics
        Extrapolation of test results between species can be aided 
    considerably by data on the pharmacokinetics of a particular agent in 
    the species tested and, if possible, in humans. Information on a 
    toxicant's half-life, metabolism, absorption, excretion, and 
    distribution to the peripheral and central nervous system may be useful 
    in predicting risk. Of particular importance for the pharmacokinetics 
    of neurotoxicants is the blood-brain barrier. The vast majority of the 
    central nervous system is served by blood vessels with blood-brain 
    barrier properties, which exclude most ionic and nonlipid-soluble 
    chemicals from the brain and spinal cord. The brain contains several 
    structures called circumventricular organs (CVOs) that are served by 
    blood vessels lacking blood-brain barrier properties. Brain regions 
    adjacent to these CVOs are thus exposed to relatively high levels of 
    many neurotoxicants. Pharmacokinetic data may be helpful in defining 
    the dose-response curve, developing a more accurate basis for comparing 
    species sensitivity (including that of humans), determining dosimetry 
    at sites, and comparing pharmacokinetic profiles for various dosing 
    regimens or routes of administration. The correlation of 
    pharmacokinetic parameters and neurotoxicity data may be useful in 
    determining the contribution of specific pharmacokinetic processes to 
    the effects observed.
    3.1.3.2. Comparisons of Molecular Structure
        Comparisons of the chemical or physical properties of an agent with 
    those of known neurotoxicants may provide some indication of the 
    potential for neurotoxicity. Such information may be helpful for 
    evaluating potential toxicity when only minimal data are available. The 
    structure-activity relationships (SAR) of some chemical classes have 
    been studied, including hexacarbons, organophosphates, carbamates, and 
    pyrethroids. Therefore, class relationships or SAR may help
    
    [[Page 26944]]
    
    predict neurotoxicity or interpret data from neurotoxicological 
    studies. Under certain circumstances (e.g., in the case of new 
    chemicals), this procedure is one of the primary methods used to 
    evaluate the potential for toxicity when little or no empirical 
    toxicity data are available. It should be recognized, however, that 
    effects of chemicals in the same class can vary widely. Moser (1995), 
    for example, reported that the behavioral effects of prototypic 
    cholinesterase-inhibiting pesticides differed qualitatively in a 
    battery of behavioral tests.
    3.1.3.3. Statistical Considerations
        Properly designed studies on the neurotoxic effects of compounds 
    will include appropriate statistical tests of significance. In general, 
    the likelihood of obtaining a significant effect will depend jointly on 
    the magnitude of the effect and the variability obtained in control and 
    treated groups. The risk assessor should be aware that some 
    neurotoxicants may induce a greater variability in biologic response, 
    rather than a clear shift in mean or other parameters (Laties and 
    Evans, 1980; Glowa and MacPhail, 1995). A number of texts are available 
    on standard statistical tests (e.g., Siegel, 1956; Winer, 1971; Sokal 
    and Rohlf, 1969; Salsburg, 1986; Gad and Weil, 1988).
        Neurotoxicity data present some unique features that should be 
    considered in selecting statistical tests for analysis. Data may 
    involve several different measurement scales, including categorical 
    (affected or not), rank (more or less affected), and interval and ratio 
    scales of measurement (affected by some percentage). For example, 
    convulsions are usually recorded as being present or absent 
    (categorical), whereas neuropathological changes are frequently 
    described in terms of the degree of damage (rank). Many tests of 
    neurotoxicity involve interval or ratio measurements (e.g., frequency 
    of photocell interruptions or amplitude of an evoked potential), which 
    are the most powerful and sensitive scales of measurement. In addition, 
    measurements are frequently made repeatedly in control and treated 
    subjects, especially in the case of behavioral and neurophysiological 
    endpoints. For example, OPPTS guidelines for FOB assessment call for 
    evaluations before exposure and at several times during exposure in a 
    subchronic study (U.S. EPA, 1991a).
        Descriptive data (categorical) and rank order data can be analyzed 
    using standard nonparametric techniques (Siegel, 1956). In some cases, 
    if it is determined that the data fit the linear model, the categorical 
    modeling procedure can be used for weighted least-squares estimation of 
    parameters for a wide range of general linear models, including 
    repeated-measures analyses. The weighted least-squares approach to 
    categorical and rank data allows computation of statistics for testing 
    the significance of sources of variation as reflected by the model. In 
    the case of studies assessing effects in the same animals at several 
    time points, univariate analyses can be carried out at each time point 
    when the overall dose effect or the dose-by-time interaction is 
    significant.
        Continuous data (e.g., magnitude, rate, amplitude), if found to be 
    normally distributed, can be analyzed with general linear models using 
    a grouping factor of dose and, if necessary, repeated measures across 
    time (Winer, 1971). Univariate analyses of dose, comparing dose groups 
    to the control group at each time point, can be performed when there is 
    a significant overall dose effect or a dose-by-time interaction. Post 
    hoc comparisons between control and treatment groups can be made 
    following tests for overall significance. In the case of multiple 
    endpoints within a series of evaluations, some type of correction for 
    multiple observations is warranted (Winer, 1971).
    3.1.3.4. In Vitro Data in Neurotoxicology
        Methods and procedures that fall under the general heading of 
    short-term tests include an array of in vitro tests that have been 
    proposed as alternatives to whole-animal tests (Goldberg and Frazier, 
    1989). In vitro approaches use animal or human cells, tissues, or 
    organs and maintain them in a nutritive medium. Various types of in 
    vitro techniques, including primary cell cultures, cell lines, and 
    cloned cells, produce data for evaluating potential and known 
    neurotoxic substances. While such procedures are important in studying 
    the mechanism of action of toxic agents, their use in hazard 
    identification in human health risk assessment has not been explored to 
    any great extent.
        Data from in vitro procedures are generally based on simplified 
    approaches that require less time to yield information than do many in 
    vivo techniques. However, in vitro methods generally do not take into 
    account the distribution of the toxicant in the body, the route of 
    administration, or the metabolism of the substance. It also is 
    difficult to extrapolate in vitro data to animal or human neurotoxicity 
    endpoints, which include behavioral changes, motor disorders, sensory 
    and perceptual disorders, lack of coordination, and learning deficits. 
    In addition, data from in vitro tests cannot duplicate the complex 
    neuronal circuitry characteristic of the intact animal.
        Many in vitro systems are now being evaluated for their ability to 
    predict the neurotoxicity of various agents seen in intact animals. 
    This validation process requires considerations in study design, 
    including defined endpoints of toxicity and an understanding of how a 
    test agent would be handled in vitro as compared to the intact 
    organism. Demonstrated neurotoxicity in vitro in the absence of in vivo 
    data is suggestive but inadequate evidence of a neurotoxic effect. In 
    vivo data supported by in vitro data enhance the reliability of the in 
    vivo results.
    3.1.3.5. Neuroendocrine Effects
        Neuroendocrine dysfunction may occur because of a disturbance in 
    the regulation and modulation of neuroendocrine feedback systems. One 
    major indicator of neuroendocrine function is secretion of hormones 
    from the pituitary. Hypothalamic control of anterior pituitary 
    secretions is also involved in a number of important bodily functions. 
    Many types of behaviors (e.g., reproductive behaviors, sexually 
    dimorphic behaviors in animals) are dependent on the integrity of the 
    hypothalamic-pituitary system, which could represent a potential site 
    of neurotoxicity. Pituitary secretions arise from a number of different 
    cell types in this gland, and neurotoxicants could affect these cells 
    directly or indirectly. Morphological changes in cells mediating 
    neuroendocrine secretions could be associated with adverse effects on 
    the pituitary or hypothalamus and could ultimately affect behavior and 
    the functioning of the nervous system. Biochemical changes in the 
    hypothalamus may also be used as indicators of potential adverse 
    effects on neuroendocrine function. Finally, the development of the 
    nervous system is intimately associated with the presence of 
    circulating hormones such as thyroid hormone (Porterfield, 1994). The 
    nature of the nervous system deficit, which could include cognitive 
    dysfunction, altered neurological development, or visual deficits, 
    depends on the severity of the thyroid disturbance and the specific 
    developmental period when exposure to the chemical occurred.
    3.2. Dose-Response Evaluation
        Dose-response evaluation is a critical part of the qualitative 
    characterization of a chemical's potential to produce neurotoxicity and 
    involves the description of the dose-response
    
    [[Page 26945]]
    
    relationship in the available data. Human studies covering a range of 
    exposures are rarely available, and therefore animal data are typically 
    used for estimating exposure levels likely to produce adverse effects 
    in humans. Evidence for a dose-response relationship is an important 
    criterion in establishing a neurotoxic effect, although this analysis 
    may be limited when based on standard studies using three dose groups 
    or fewer. The evaluation of dose-response relationships includes 
    identifying effective dose levels as well as doses associated with no 
    increase in adverse effects when compared with controls. The lack of a 
    dose-response relationship in the data may suggest that the effect is 
    not related to the putative neurotoxic effect or that the study was not 
    appropriately controlled. Much of the focus is on identifying the 
    critical effect(s) observed at the LOAEL and the NOAEL associated with 
    that effect. The NOAEL is defined as the highest dose at which there is 
    no statistically or biologically significant increase in the frequency 
    of an adverse neurotoxic effect when compared with the appropriate 
    control group in a database characterized as having sufficient evidence 
    for use in a risk assessment (see section 3.3). The risk assessor 
    should be aware of possible problems associated with estimating a NOAEL 
    in studies involving a small number of test subjects and that have a 
    poor dose-response relationship.
        In addition to identifying the NOAEL/LOAEL or BMD, the dose-
    response evaluation defines the range of doses that are neurotoxic for 
    a given agent, species, route of exposure, and duration of exposure. In 
    addition to these considerations, pharmacokinetic factors and other 
    aspects that might influence comparisons with human exposure scenarios 
    should be taken into account. For example, dose-response curves may 
    exhibit not only monotonic but also U-shaped or inverted U-shaped 
    functions (Davis and Svendsgaard, 1990). Such curves are hypothesized 
    to reflect multiple mechanisms of action, the presence of homeostatic 
    mechanisms, and/or activation of compensatory or protective mechanisms. 
    In addition to considering the shape of the dose-response curve, it 
    should also be recognized that neurotoxic effects vary in terms of 
    nature and severity across dose or exposure level. At high levels of 
    exposure, frank lesions accompanied by severe functional impairment may 
    be observed. Such effects are widely accepted as adverse. At 
    progressively lower levels of exposure, however, the lesions may become 
    less severe and the impairments less obvious. At levels of exposure 
    near the NOAEL and LOAEL, the effects will often be mild, possibly 
    reversible, and inconsistently found. In addition, the endpoints 
    showing responses may be at levels of organization below the whole 
    organism (e.g., neurochemical or electrophysiological endpoints). The 
    adversity of such effects can be disputed (e.g., cholinesterase 
    inhibition), yet it is such effects that are likely to be the focus of 
    risk assessment decisions. To the extent possible, this document 
    provides guidance on determining the adversity of neurotoxic effects. 
    However, the identification of a critical adverse effect often requires 
    considerable professional judgment and should consider factors such as 
    the biological plausibility of the effect, the evidence of a dose-
    effect continuum, and the likelihood for progression of the effect with 
    continued exposure.
    3.3. Characterization of the Health-Related Database
        This section describes a scheme for characterizing the sufficiency 
    of evidence for neurotoxic effects. This scheme defines two broad 
    categories: sufficient and insufficient (Table 8). Categorization is 
    aimed at providing certain criteria for the Agency to use to define the 
    minimum evidence necessary to define hazards and to conduct dose-
    response analyses. It does not address the issues related to 
    characterization of risk, which requires analysis of potential human 
    exposures and their relation to potential hazards in order to estimate 
    the risks of those hazards from anticipated or estimated exposures. 
    Several examples using a weight-of-evidence approach similar to that 
    described in these Guidelines have been described elsewhere (Tilson et 
    al., 1995; Tilson et al., 1996).
    
                                Table 8.--Characterization of the Health-Related Database                           
    ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
                                                                                                                    
    ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
    Sufficient evidence.................................  The sufficient evidence category includes data that       
                                                           collectively provide enough information to judge whether 
                                                           or not a human neurotoxic hazard could exist. This       
                                                           category may include both human and experimental animal  
                                                           evidence.                                                
    Sufficient human evidence...........................  This category includes agents for which there is          
                                                           sufficient evidence from epidemiologic studies, e.g.,    
                                                           case control and cohort studies, to judge that some      
                                                           neurotoxic effect is associated with exposure. A case    
                                                           series in conjunction with other supporting evidence may 
                                                           also be judged ``sufficient evidence.'' Epidemiologic and
                                                           clinical case studies should discuss whether the observed
                                                           effects can be considered biologically plausible in      
                                                           relation to chemical exposure. (Historically, often much 
                                                           has been made of the notion of causality in epidemiologic
                                                           studies. Causality is a more stringent criterion than    
                                                           association and has become a topic of scientific and     
                                                           philosophical debate. See Susser [1986], for example, for
                                                           a discussion of inference in epidemiology.)              
    Sufficient experimental animal evidence/limited       This category includes agents for which there is          
     human data.                                           sufficient evidence from experimental animal studies and/
                                                           or limited human data to judge whether a potential       
                                                           neurotoxic hazard may exist. Generally, agents that have 
                                                           been tested according to current test guidelines would be
                                                           included in this category. The minimum evidence necessary
                                                           to judge that a potential hazard exists would be data    
                                                           demonstrating an adverse neurotoxic effect in a single   
                                                           appropriate, well-executed study in a single experimental
                                                           animal species. The minimum evidence needed to judge that
                                                           a potential hazard does not exist would include data from
                                                           an appropriate number of endpoints from more than one    
                                                           study and two species showing no adverse neurotoxic      
                                                           effects at doses that were minimally toxic in terms of   
                                                           producing an adverse effect. Information on              
                                                           pharmacokinetics, mechanisms, or known properties of the 
                                                           chemical class may also strengthen the evidence.         
    
    [[Page 26946]]
    
                                                                                                                    
    Insufficient evidence...............................  This category includes agents for which there is less than
                                                           the minimum evidence sufficient for identifying whether  
                                                           or not a neurotoxic hazard exists, such as agents for    
                                                           which there are no data on neurotoxicity or agents with  
                                                           databases from studies in animals or humans that are     
                                                           limited by study design or conduct (e.g., inadequate     
                                                           conduct or report of clinical signs). Many general       
                                                           toxicity studies, for example, are considered            
                                                           insufficient in terms of the conduct of clinical         
                                                           neurobehavioral observations or the number of samples    
                                                           taken for histopathology of the nervous system. Thus, a  
                                                           battery of negative toxicity studies with these          
                                                           shortcomings would be regarded as providing insufficient 
                                                           evidence of the lack of a neurotoxic effect of the test  
                                                           material. Further, most screening studies based on simple
                                                           observations involving autonomic and motor function      
                                                           provide insufficient evaluation of many sensory or       
                                                           cognitive functions. Data, which by itself would likely  
                                                           fall in this category, would also include information on 
                                                           SAR or data from in vitro tests. Although such           
                                                           information would be insufficient by itself to proceed   
                                                           further in the assessment it could be used to support the
                                                           need for additional testing.                             
    ----------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------------
    
        Data from all potentially relevant studies, whether indicative of 
    potential hazard or not, should be included in this characterization. 
    The primary sources of data are human studies and case reports, 
    experimental animal studies, other supporting data, and in vitro and/or 
    SAR data. Because a complex interrelationship exists among study 
    design, statistical analysis, and biological significance of the data, 
    a great deal of scientific judgment, based on experience with 
    neurotoxicity data and with the principles of study design and 
    statistical analysis, is required to adequately evaluate the database 
    on neurotoxicity. In many cases, interaction with scientists in 
    specific disciplines either within or outside the field of 
    neurotoxicology (e.g., epidemiology, statistics) may be appropriate.
        The adverse nature of different neurotoxicity endpoints may be a 
    complex judgment. In general, most neuropathological and many 
    neurobehavioral changes are regarded as adverse. However, there are 
    adverse behavioral effects that may not reflect a direct action on the 
    nervous system. Neurochemical and electrophysiological changes may be 
    regarded as adverse because of their known or presumed relation to 
    neuropathological and/or neurobehavioral consequences. In the absence 
    of supportive information, a professional judgment should be made 
    regarding the adversity of such outcomes, considering factors such as 
    the nature, magnitude, and duration of the effects reported. Thus, 
    correlated measures of neurotoxicity strengthen the evidence for a 
    hazard. Correlations between functional and morphological effects, such 
    as the correlation between leg weakness and paralysis and peripheral 
    nerve damage from exposure to tri-ortho-cresyl phosphate, are the most 
    common and striking examples of this form of validity. Correlations 
    support a coherent and logical link between behavioral effects and 
    biochemical mechanisms. Replication of a finding also strengthens the 
    evidence for a hazard. Some neurotoxicants cause similar effects across 
    most species. Many chemicals shown to produce neurotoxicity in 
    laboratory animals have similar effects in humans. Some neurological 
    effects may be considered adverse even if they are small in magnitude, 
    reversible, or the result of indirect mechanisms.
        Because of the inherent difficulty in ``proving any negative,'' it 
    is more difficult to document a finding of no apparent adverse effect 
    than a finding of an adverse effect. Neurotoxic effects (and most kinds 
    of toxicity) can be observed at many different levels, so only a single 
    endpoint needs to be found to demonstrate a hazard, but many endpoints 
    need to be examined to demonstrate no effect. For example, to judge 
    that a hazard for neurotoxicity could exist for a given agent, the 
    minimum evidence sufficient would be data on a single adverse endpoint 
    from a well-conducted study. In contrast, to judge that an agent is 
    unlikely to pose a hazard for neurotoxicity, the minimum evidence would 
    include data from a host of endpoints that revealed no neurotoxic 
    effects. This may include human data from appropriate studies that 
    could support a conclusion of no evidence of a neurotoxic effect. With 
    respect to clinical signs and symptoms, human exposures can reveal far 
    more about the absence of effects than animal studies, which are 
    confined to the signs examined.
        In some cases, it may be that no individual study is judged 
    sufficient to establish a hazard, but the total available data may 
    support such a conclusion. Pharmacokinetic data and structure-activity 
    considerations, data from other toxicity studies, or other factors may 
    affect the strength of the evidence in these situations. For example, 
    given that gamma diketones are known to cause motor system 
    neurotoxicity, a marginal data set on a candidate gamma diketone, e.g., 
    1/10 animals affected, might be more likely to be judged sufficient 
    than equivalent data from a member of a chemical class about which 
    nothing is known.
        A judgment that the toxicology database is sufficient to indicate a 
    potential neurotoxic hazard is not the end of analysis. The 
    circumstances of expression of the hazard are essential to describing 
    human hazard potential. Thus, reporting should contain the details of 
    the circumstances under which effects have been observed, e.g., ``long-
    term oral exposures of adult rodents to compound X at levels of roughly 
    1 mg/kg have been associated with ataxia and peripheral nerve damage.''
    
    4. Quantitative Dose-Response Analysis
    
        This section describes several approaches (including the LOAEL/
    NOAEL and BMD) for determining the reference dose (RfD) or reference 
    concentration (RfC). The NOAEL or BMD/uncertainty factor approach 
    results in an RfD or RfC, which is an estimate (with uncertainty 
    spanning perhaps an order of magnitude) of a daily exposure to the 
    human population (including sensitive subgroups) that is likely to be 
    without an appreciable risk of deleterious effects during a lifetime.
        The dose-response analysis characterization should:
         Describe how the RfD/RfC was calculated;
         Discuss the confidence in the estimates;
         Describe the assumptions or uncertainty factors used; and
         Discuss the route and level of exposure observed, as 
    compared to expected human exposures.
    4.1. LOAEL/NOAEL and BMD Determination
        As indicated earlier, the LOAEL and NOAEL are determined for 
    endpoints that are seen at the lowest dose level (so-called critical 
    effect). Several limitations in the use of the NOAEL have been 
    identified and described (e.g., Barnes and Dourson, 1988; Crump, 1984). 
    For example, the NOAEL is derived from a single endpoint from a single 
    study (the critical study) and
    
    [[Page 26947]]
    
    ignores both the slope of the dose-response function and baseline 
    variability in the endpoint of concern. Because the baseline 
    variability is not taken into account, the NOAEL from a study using 
    small group sizes may be higher than the NOAEL from a similar study in 
    the same species that uses larger group sizes. The NOAEL is also 
    directly dependent on the dose spacing used in the study. Finally, and 
    perhaps most importantly, use of the NOAEL does not allow estimates of 
    risk or extrapolation of risk to lower dose levels. Because of these 
    and other limitations in the NOAEL approach, it has been proposed that 
    mathematical curve-fitting techniques (Crump, 1984; Gaylor and Slikker, 
    1990; Glowa, 1991; Glowa and MacPhail, 1995; U.S. EPA, 1995a) be 
    compared with the NOAEL procedure in calculating the RfD or RfC. These 
    techniques typically apply a mathematical function that describes the 
    dose-response relationship and then interpolate to a level of exposure 
    associated with a small increase in effect over that occurring in the 
    control group or under baseline conditions. The BMD has been defined as 
    a lower confidence limit on the effective dose associated with some 
    defined level of effect, e.g., a 5% or 10% increase in response. These 
    guidelines suggest that the use of the BMD should be explored in 
    specific situations. The Agency is currently developing guidelines for 
    the use of the BMD in risk assessment.
        Many neurotoxic endpoints provide continuous measures of response, 
    such as response speed, nerve conduction velocity, IQ score, degree of 
    enzyme inhibition, or the accuracy of task performance. Although it is 
    possible to impose a dichotomy on a continuous effects distribution and 
    to classify some level of response as ``affected'' and the remainder as 
    ``unaffected,'' it may be very difficult and inappropriate to establish 
    such clear distinctions, because such a dichotomy would misrepresent 
    the true nature of the neurotoxic response. The risk assessor should be 
    aware of the importance of trying to reconcile findings from several 
    studies that seem to report widely divergent results. Alternatively, 
    quantitative models designed to analyze continuous effect variables may 
    be preferable. Other techniques that allow this approach, with 
    transformation of the information into estimates of the incidence or 
    frequency of affected individuals in a population, have been proposed 
    (Crump, 1984; Gaylor and Slikker, 1990; Glowa and MacPhail, 1995). 
    Categorical regression analysis has been proposed because it can 
    evaluate different types of data and derive estimates for short-term 
    exposures (Rees and Hattis, 1994). Decisions about the most appropriate 
    approach require professional judgment, taking into account the 
    biological nature of the continuous effect variable and its 
    distribution in the population under study.
        Although dose-response functions in neurotoxicology are generally 
    linear or monotonic, curvilinear functions, especially U-shaped or 
    inverted U-shaped curves, have been reported as noted earlier (section 
    3.2). Dose-response analyses should consider the uncertainty that U-
    shaped dose-response functions might contribute to the estimate of the 
    NOAEL/LOAEL or BMD. Typically, estimates of the NOAEL/LOAEL are taken 
    from the lowest part of the dose-response curve associated with 
    impaired function or adverse effect.
    4.2. Determination of the Reference Dose or Reference Concentration
        Since the availability of dose-response data in humans is limited, 
    extrapolation of data from animals to humans usually involves the 
    application of uncertainty factors to the NOAEL/LOAEL or BMD. The NOAEL 
    or BMD/uncertainty factor approach results in an RfD or RfC, which is 
    an estimate (with uncertainty spanning perhaps an order of magnitude) 
    of a daily exposure to the human population (including sensitive 
    subgroups) that is likely to be without an appreciable risk of 
    deleterious effects during a lifetime. The oral RfD and inhalation RfC 
    are applicable to chronic exposure situations and are based on an 
    evaluation of all the noncancer health effects, including neurotoxicity 
    data. RfDs and RfCs in the Integrated Risk Information System (IRIS-2) 
    database for several agents are based on neurotoxicity endpoints and 
    include a few cases in which the RfD or RfC is calculated using the BMD 
    approach (e.g., methylmercury, carbon disulfide). The size of the final 
    uncertainty factor used will vary from agent to agent and will require 
    the exercise of scientific judgment, taking into account interspecies 
    differences, the shape of the dose-response curve, and the 
    neurotoxicity endpoints observed. Uncertainty factors are typically 
    multiples of 10 and are used to compensate for human variability in 
    sensitivity, the need to extrapolate from animals to humans, and the 
    need to extrapolate from less than lifetime (e.g., subchronic) to 
    lifetime exposures. An additional factor of up to 10 may be included 
    when only a LOAEL (and not a NOAEL) is available from a study, or 
    depending on the completeness of the database, a modifying factor of up 
    to 10 may be applied, depending on the confidence one has in the 
    database. Uncertainty factors of less than 10 can be used, depending 
    upon the availability of relevant information. Barnes and Dourson 
    (1988) provide a more complete description of the calculation, use, and 
    significance of RfDs in setting exposure limits to toxic agents by the 
    oral route. Jarabek et al. (1990) provide a more complete description 
    of the calculation, use, and significance of RfCs in setting exposure 
    limits to toxic agents in air. Neurotoxicity can result from acute, 
    shorter term exposures, and it may be appropriate in some cases, e.g., 
    for air pollutants or water contaminants, to set shorter term exposure 
    limits for neurotoxicity as well as for other noncancer health effects.
    
    5. Exposure Assessment
    
        Exposure assessment describes the magnitude, duration, frequency, 
    and routes of exposure to the agent of interest. This information may 
    come from hypothetical values, models, or actual experimental values, 
    including ambient environmental sampling results. Guidelines for 
    exposure assessment have been published separately (U.S. EPA, 1992) and 
    will, therefore, be discussed only briefly here.
        The exposure assessment should include an exposure characterization 
    that:
         Provides a statement of the purpose, scope, level of 
    detail, and approach used in the exposure assessment;
         Presents the estimates of exposure and dose by pathway and 
    route for individuals, population segments, and populations in a manner 
    appropriate for the intended risk characterization;
         Provides an evaluation of the overall level of confidence 
    in the estimate of exposure and dose and the conclusions drawn; and
         Communicates the results of the exposure assessment to the 
    risk assessor, who can then use the exposure characterization, along 
    with the hazard and dose/response characterizations, to develop a risk 
    characterization.
        A number of considerations are relevant to exposure assessment for 
    neurotoxicants. An appropriate evaluation of exposure should consider 
    the potential for exposure via ingestion, inhalation, and dermal 
    penetration from relevant sources of exposure, including multiple 
    avenues of intake from the same source.
        In addition, neurotoxic effects may result from short-term (acute), 
    high-concentration exposures as well as from
    
    [[Page 26948]]
    
    longer term (subchronic), lower level exposures. Neurotoxic effects may 
    occur after a period of time following initial exposure or be 
    obfuscated by repair mechanisms or apparent tolerance. The type and 
    severity of effect may depend significantly on the pattern of exposure 
    rather than on the average dose over a long period of time. For this 
    reason, exposure assessments for neurotoxicants may be much more 
    complicated than those for long-latency effects such as 
    carcinogenicity. It is rare for sufficient data to be available to 
    construct such patterns of exposure or dose, and professional judgment 
    may be necessary to evaluate exposure to neurotoxic agents.
    
    6. Risk Characterization
    
    6.1. Overview
        Risk characterization is the summarization step of the risk 
    assessment process and consists of an integrative analysis and a 
    summary. The integrative analysis (a) involves integration of the 
    toxicity information from the hazard characterization and dose-response 
    analysis with the human exposure estimates, (b) provides an evaluation 
    of the overall quality of the assessment and the degree of confidence 
    in the estimates of risk and conclusions drawn, and  
    describes risk in terms of the nature and extent of harm. The risk 
    characterization summary communicates the results of the risk 
    assessment to the risk manager in a complete, informative, and useful 
    format.
        This summary should include, but is not limited to, a discussion of 
    the following elements:
         Quality of and confidence in the available data;
         Uncertainty analysis;
         Justification of defaults or assumptions;
         Related research recommendations;
         Contentious issues and extent of scientific consensus;
         Effect of reasonable alternative assumptions on 
    conclusions and estimates;
         Highlights of reasonable plausible ranges;
         Reasonable alternative models; and
         Perspectives through analogy.
        The risk manager can then use the derived risk to make public 
    health decisions.
        An effective risk characterization should fully, openly, and 
    clearly characterize risks and disclose the scientific analyses, 
    uncertainties, assumptions, and science policies that underlie 
    decisions throughout the risk assessment and risk management processes. 
    The risk characterization should feature values such as transparency in 
    the decision-making process; clarity in communicating with the 
    scientific community and the public regarding environmental risk and 
    the uncertainties associated with assessments of environmental risk; 
    and consistency across program offices in core assumptions and science 
    policies, which are well grounded in science and reasonable. The 
    following sections describe these four aspects of the risk 
    characterization in more detail.
    6.2. Integration of Hazard Characterization, Dose-Response Analysis, 
    and Exposure Assessment
        In developing the hazard characterization, dose-response analysis, 
    and exposure portions of the risk assessment, the risk assessor should 
    take into account many judgments concerning human relevance of the 
    toxicity data, including the appropriateness of the various animal 
    models for which data are available and the route, timing, and duration 
    of exposure relative to expected human exposure. These judgments should 
    be summarized at each stage of the risk assessment process (e.g., the 
    biological relevance of anatomical variations may be established in the 
    hazard characterization process, or the influence of species 
    differences in metabolic patterns in the dose-response analysis). In 
    integrating the information from the assessment, the risk assessor 
    should determine if some of these judgments have implications for other 
    portions of the assessment and whether the various components of the 
    assessment are compatible.
        The risk characterization should not only examine the judgments but 
    also explain the constraints of available data and the state of 
    knowledge about the phenomena studied in making them, including (1) the 
    qualitative conclusions about the likelihood that the chemical may pose 
    a specific hazard to human health, the nature of the observed effects, 
    under what conditions (route, dose levels, time, and duration) of 
    exposure these effects occur, and whether the health-related data are 
    sufficient to use in a risk assessment; (2) a discussion of the dose-
    response characteristics of the critical effects, data such as the 
    shapes and slopes of the dose-response curves for the various 
    endpoints, the rationale behind the determination of the NOAEL and 
    LOAEL and calculation of the benchmark dose, and the assumptions 
    underlying the estimation of the RfD or RfC; and (3) the estimates of 
    the magnitude of human exposure; the route, duration, and pattern of 
    the exposure; relevant pharmacokinetics; and the number and 
    characteristics of the population(s) exposed.
        If data to be used in a risk characterization are from a route of 
    exposure other than the expected human exposure, then pharmacokinetic 
    data should be used, if available, to make extrapolations across routes 
    of exposure. If such data are not available, the Agency makes certain 
    assumptions concerning the amount of absorption likely or the 
    applicability of the data from one route to another (U.S. EPA, 1992).
        The level of confidence in the hazard characterization should be 
    stated to the extent possible, including the appropriate category 
    regarding sufficiency of the health-related data. A comprehensive risk 
    assessment ideally includes information on a variety of endpoints that 
    provide insight into the full spectrum of potential neurotoxicological 
    responses. A profile that integrates both human and test species data 
    and incorporates a broad range of potential adverse neurotoxic effects 
    provides more confidence in a risk assessment for a given agent.
        The ability to describe the nature of the potential human exposure 
    is important in order to predict when certain outcomes can be 
    anticipated and the likelihood of permanence or reversibility of the 
    effect. An important part of this effort is a description of the nature 
    of the exposed population and the potential for sensitive, highly 
    susceptible, or highly exposed populations. For example, the 
    consequences of exposure to the developing individual versus the adult 
    can differ markedly and can influence whether the effects are transient 
    or permanent. Other considerations relative to human exposures might 
    include the likelihood of exposures to other agents, concurrent 
    disease, and nutritional status.
        The presentation of the integrated results of the assessment should 
    draw from and highlight key points of the individual characterizations 
    of component analyses performed under these Guidelines. The overall 
    risk characterization represents the integration of these component 
    characterizations. If relevant risk assessments on the agent or an 
    analogous agent have been done by EPA or other Federal agencies, these 
    should be described and the similarities and differences discussed.
    
    [[Page 26949]]
    
    6.3. Quality of the Database and Degree of Confidence in the Assessment
        The risk characterization should summarize the kinds of data 
    brought together in the analysis and the reasoning on which the 
    assessment is based. The description should convey the major strengths 
    and weaknesses of the assessment that arise from availability of data 
    and the current limits of our understanding of the mechanisms of 
    toxicity.
        A health risk assessment is only as good as its component parts, 
    i.e., hazard characterization, dose-response analysis, and exposure 
    assessment. Confidence in the results of a risk assessment is thus a 
    function of confidence in the results of the analysis of these 
    elements. Each of these elements should have its own characterization 
    as a part of the assessment. Within each characterization, the 
    important uncertainties of the analysis and interpretation of data 
    should be explained, and the risk manager should be given a clear 
    picture of consensus or lack of consensus that exists about significant 
    aspects of the assessment. Whenever more than one view is supported by 
    the data and choosing between them is difficult, all views should be 
    presented. If one has been selected over the others, the rationale 
    should be given; if not, then all should be presented as plausible 
    alternative results.
    6.4. Descriptors of Neurotoxicity Risk
        There are a number of ways to describe risks. Several relevant ways 
    for neurotoxicity are as follows:
    6.4.1. Estimation of the Number of Individuals
        The RfD or RfC is taken to be a chronic exposure level at or below 
    which no significant risk occurs. Therefore, presentation of the 
    population in terms of those at or below the RfD or RfC (``not at 
    risk'') and above the RfD or RfC (``may be at risk'') may be useful 
    information for risk managers. This method is particularly useful to a 
    risk manager considering possible actions to ameliorate risk for a 
    population. If the number of persons in the at-risk category can be 
    estimated, then the number of persons removed from the at-risk category 
    after a contemplated action is taken can be used as an indication of 
    the efficacy of the action.
    6.4.2. Presentation of Specific Scenarios
        Presenting specific scenarios in the form of ``what if?'' questions 
    is particularly useful to give perspective to the risk manager, 
    especially where criteria, tolerance limits, or media quality limits 
    are being set. The question being asked in these cases is, at this 
    proposed exposure limit, what would be the resulting risk for 
    neurotoxicity above the RfD or RfC?
    6.4.3. Risk Characterization for Highly Exposed Individuals
        This measure is one example of the just-discussed descriptor. This 
    measure describes the magnitude of concern at the upper end of the 
    exposure distribution. This allows risk managers to evaluate whether 
    certain individuals are at disproportionately high or unacceptably high 
    risk.
        The objective of looking at the upper end of the exposure 
    distribution is to derive a realistic estimate of a relatively highly 
    exposed individual or individuals. This measure could be addressed by 
    identifying a specified upper percentile of exposure in the population 
    and/or by estimating the exposure of the highest exposed individual(s). 
    Whenever possible, it is important to express the number of individuals 
    who comprise the selected highly exposed group and discuss the 
    potential for exposure at still higher levels.
        If population data are absent, it will often be possible to 
    describe a scenario representing high-end exposures using upper 
    percentile or judgment-based values for exposure variables. In these 
    instances caution should be used in order not to compound a substantial 
    number of high-end values for variables if a ``reasonable'' exposure 
    estimate is to be achieved.
    6.4.4. Risk Characterization for Highly Sensitive or Susceptible 
    Individuals
        This measure identifies populations sensitive or susceptible to the 
    effect of concern. Sensitive or susceptible individuals are those 
    within the exposed population at increased risk of expressing the toxic 
    effect. All stages of nervous system maturation might be considered 
    highly sensitive or susceptible, but certain subpopulations can 
    sometimes be identified because of critical periods for exposure, for 
    example, pregnant or lactating women, infants, or children. The aged 
    population is considered to be at particular risk because of the 
    limited ability of the nervous system to regenerate or compensate to 
    neurotoxic insult.
        In general, not enough is understood about the mechanisms of 
    toxicity to identify sensitive subgroups for all agents, although 
    factors such as nutrition (e.g., vitamin B), personal habits (e.g., 
    smoking, alcohol consumption, illicit drug abuse), or preexisting 
    disease (e.g., diabetes, neurological diseases, sexually transmitted 
    diseases, polymorphisms for certain metabolic enzymes) may predispose 
    some individuals to be more sensitive to the neurotoxic effects of 
    specific agents. Gender-related differences in response to 
    neurotoxicants have been noted, but these appear to be related to 
    gender-dependent toxicodynamic or toxicokinetic factors.
        In general, it is assumed that an uncertainty factor of 10 for 
    intrapopulation variability will be able to accommodate differences in 
    sensitivity among various subpopulations, including children and the 
    elderly. However, in cases where it can be demonstrated that a factor 
    of 10 does not afford adequate protection, another uncertainty factor 
    may be considered in conducting the risk assessment.
    6.4.5. Other Risk Descriptors
        In risk characterization, dose-response information and the human 
    exposure estimates may be combined either by comparing the RfD or RfC 
    and the human exposure estimate or by calculating the margin of 
    exposure (MOE). The MOE is the ratio of the NOAEL from the most 
    appropriate or sensitive species to the estimated human exposure level. 
    If a NOAEL is not available, a LOAEL may be used in calculating the 
    MOE. Alternatively, a benchmark dose may be compared with the estimated 
    human exposure level to obtain the MOE. Considerations for the 
    evaluation of the MOE are similar to those for the uncertainty factor 
    applied to the LOAEL/NOAEL or the benchmark dose. The MOE is presented 
    along with a discussion of the adequacy of the database, including the 
    nature and quality of the hazard and exposure data, the number of 
    species affected, and the dose-response information.
        The RfD or RfC comparison with the human exposure estimate and the 
    calculation of the MOE are conceptually similar but are used in 
    different regulatory situations. The choice of approach depends on 
    several factors, including the statute involved, the situation being 
    addressed, the database used, and the needs of the decision maker. The 
    RfD or RfC and the MOE are considered along with other risk assessment 
    and risk management issues in making risk management decisions, but the 
    scientific issues that should be taken into account in establishing 
    them have been addressed here.
    
    [[Page 26950]]
    
        If the MOE is equal to or more than the uncertainty factor 
    multiplied by any modifying factor used as a basis for an RfD or RfC, 
    then the need for regulatory concern is likely to be small. Although 
    these methods of describing risk do not actually estimate risks per se, 
    they give the risk manager some sense of how close the exposures are to 
    levels of concern.
    6.5. Communicating Results
        Once the risk characterization is completed, the focus turns to 
    communicating results to the risk manager. The risk manager uses the 
    results of the risk characterization along with other technological, 
    social, and economic considerations in reaching a regulatory decision. 
    Because of the way in which these risk management factors may affect 
    different cases, consistent but not necessarily identical risk 
    management decisions should be made on a case-by-case basis. These 
    Guidelines are not intended to give guidance on the nonscientific 
    aspects of risk management decisions.
    6.6. Summary and Research Needs
        These Guidelines summarize the procedures that the U.S. 
    Environmental Protection Agency would use in evaluating the potential 
    for agents to cause neurotoxicity. These Guidelines discuss the general 
    default assumptions that should be made in risk assessment for 
    neurotoxicity because of gaps in our knowledge about underlying 
    biological processes and how these compare across species. Research to 
    improve the risk assessment process is needed in a number of areas. For 
    example, research is needed to delineate the mechanisms of 
    neurotoxicity and pathogenesis, provide comparative pharmacokinetic 
    data, examine the validity of short-term in vivo and in vitro tests, 
    elucidate the functional modalities that may be altered, develop 
    improved animal models to examine the neurotoxic effects of exposure 
    during the premating and early postmating periods and in neonates, 
    further evaluate the relationship between maternal and developmental 
    toxicity, provide insight into the concept of threshold, develop 
    approaches for improved mathematical modeling of neurotoxic effects, 
    improve animal models for examining the effects of agents given by 
    various routes of exposure, determine the effects of recurrent 
    exposures over prolonged periods of time, and address the synergistic 
    or antagonistic effects of mixed exposures and neurotoxic response. 
    Such research will aid in the evaluation and interpretation of data on 
    neurotoxicity and should provide methods to assess risk more precisely. 
    Additional research is needed to determine the most appropriate dose-
    response approach to be used in neurotoxicity risk assessments.
    
    REFERENCES
    
        Adams, J; Buelke-Sam, J. (1981) Behavioral testing of the 
    postnatal animal: testing and methods development. In: Kimmel, CA; 
    Buelke-Sam, J, eds. Developmental toxicology. New York: Raven Press, 
    pp. 233-238.
        Albert, A. (1973) Selective toxicity: the physico-chemical basis 
    of therapy. New York: Wiley, pp. 173-211.
        Anger, WK. (1984) Neurobehavioral testing of chemicals: impact 
    on recommended standards. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol 6:147-153.
        Anger, WK. (1986) Workplace exposures. In: Annau, ZA, ed., 
    Neurobehavioral toxicology. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University 
    Press, pp. 331-347.
        Anger, WK. (1990) Worksite behavioral research: results, 
    sensitive methods, test batteries, and the transition from 
    laboratory data to human health. Neurotoxicology 11:627-718.
        Anger, K; Johnson, BL. (1985) Chemicals affecting behavior. In: 
    O'Donoghue, J, ed. Neurotoxicity of industrial and commercial 
    chemicals. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
        Anger, WK; Otto, DA; Letz, R. (Eds) (1996) Symposium on 
    computerized behavioral testing of humans in neurotoxicology 
    research. Neurotoxicol Teratol 18:347-518.
        Baker, ER; Letz, R; Fidler, A; Shalat, S; Plantamura, D; Lyndon, 
    M. (1985) Computer-based neurobehavioral testing for occupational 
    and environmental epidemiology: Methodology and validation studies. 
    Neurobehav Teratol 7:369-377.
        Barone, S; Stanton, ME; Mundy, WR. (1995) Neurotoxic effects of 
    neonatal triethyl tin (TET) exposure are exacerbated with aging. 
    Neurobiol Aging 16:723-735.
        Barnes, DG; Dourson, M. (1988) Reference dose (RfD): description 
    and use in health risk assessments. Regulat Toxicol Pharmacol 8:471-
    486.
        Benignus, VA. (1993) Importance of experimenter-blind procedure 
    in neurotoxicology. Neurotoxicol Teratol 15:45-49.
        Bondy, SC. (1986) The biochemical evaluation of neurotoxic 
    damage. Fundam Appl Toxicol 6:208-216.
        Boyes, WK. (1992) Testing visual system toxicity using visual 
    evoked potential technology. In: Isaacson, RL; Jensen, KF, eds., The 
    vulnerable brain and environmental risks, Vol. 1: Malnutrition and 
    hazard assessment. New York: Plenum, pp. 193-222.
        Boyes, WK. (1993) Sensory-evoked potentials: measures of 
    neurotoxicity. In: Erinoff, L, ed. Assessing the toxicity of drugs 
    of abuse. NIDA Research Monograph 136. National Institute on Drug 
    Abuse, Alcohol, Drug Abuse and Mental Health Administration, U.S. 
    Department of Health and Human Services, pp. 63-100.
        Buelke-Sam, J; Kimmel, CA; Adams, J. (1985) Design 
    considerations in screening for behavioral teratogens: results of 
    the collaborative teratology study. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol 
    7:537-589.
        Bushnell, P; Padilla, S; Ward, T; Pope, C; Olszyk, V. (1991) 
    Behavioral and neurochemical changes in rats dosed repeatedly with 
    diisopropylfluorophosphate (DFP). J Pharmacol Exp Therap 256:741-
    750.
        Callender, TJ; Morrow, L; Subramanian, K. (1994) Evaluation of 
    chronic neurological sequelae after acute pesticide exposure using 
    SPECT brain scans. J Toxicol Environ Health 41:275-284.
        Carson, BL; Stockton, RA; Wilkinson, RR. (1987) Organomercury, 
    lead, tin compounds in the environment and the potential for human 
    exposure. In: Tilson, HA; Sparber, SB, eds. Neurotoxicants and 
    neurobiological function: Effects of organoheavy metals. New York: 
    J. Wiley, pp. 1-80.
        Coles, CD; Brown, RT; Smith, IE; Platzman, KA; Erickson, S; 
    Falek, A. (1991) Effects of prenatal alcohol exposure at school age 
    I. Physical and cognitive development. Neurotoxicol Teratol 13:357-
    367.
        Cory-Slechta, DA. (1989) Behavioral measures of neurotoxicity. 
    Neurotoxicology 10:271-296.
        Costa, LG. (1988) Interactions of neurotoxicants with 
    neurotransmitter systems. Toxicology 49:359-366.
        Crump, KS. (1984) A new method for determining allowable daily 
    intakes. Fundam Appl Toxicol 4:854-871.
        Davis, JM; Svendsgaard, DJ. (1990) U-shaped dose-response 
    curves: their occurrence and implication for risk assessment. J 
    Toxicol Environ Health 30:71-83.
        Dyer, RS. (1985) The use of sensory evoked potentials in 
    toxicology. Fundam Appl Toxicol 5:24-40.
        Dyer, RS. (1987) Macrophysiological assessment of organometal 
    neurotoxicity. In: Tilson, HA; Sparber, SB, eds. Neurotoxicants and 
    neurobiological function effects of organoheavy metals. New York: J. 
    Wiley, pp. 137-184.
        Eccles, CU. (1988) EEG correlates of neurotoxicity. Neurotoxicol 
    Teratol 10:423-428.
        Ecobichon, DJ; Joy, RM. (1982) Pesticides and neurological 
    diseases. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press, pp. 151-203.
        Ecobichon, DJ; Davies, JE; Doull, J; Ehrich, M; Joy, R; 
    McMillan, D; MacPhail, R; Reiter, LW; Slikker, W, Jr.; Tilson, H. 
    (1990) Neurotoxic effects of pesticides. In: Baker, SR; Wilkinson, 
    CF, eds. The effect of pesticides on human health. Princeton, NJ: 
    Princeton Scientific Publishing Co., Inc., pp. 131-199.
        Ehrich, M; Jortner, BS; Padilla, S. (1995) Comparison of the 
    relative inhibition of acetylcholinesterase and neuropathy esterase 
    in rats and hens given cholinesterase inhibitors. Fundam Appl 
    Toxicol 24:94-101.
        Eldefrawi, AT; Jett, D; Eldefrawi, ME. (1992) Direct actions of 
    organophosphorus anticholinesterases on muscarinic receptors. In: 
    Chambers, JE; Levi, PE, eds. Organophosphates: chemistry, fate and 
    effects. New York, Academic Press, pp. 257-270.
        Friedlander, BR; Hearn, HT. (1980) Epidemiologic considerations 
    in studying neurotoxic disease. In: Spencer, PS;
    
    [[Page 26951]]
    
    Schaumberg, HH, eds. Experimental and clinical neurotoxicology. 
    Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, pp. 650-662.
        Gad, SC. (1982) A neuromuscular screen for use in industrial 
    toxicology. J Toxicol Environ Health 9:691-704.
        Gad, S; Weil, CS. (1988) Statistics and experimental design for 
    toxicologists, 2nd ed. Caldwell, NJ: Telford Press.
        Gaylor, DW; Slikker, W. (1990) Risk assessment for neurotoxic 
    effects. Neurotoxicology 11:211-218.
        Glowa, JR. (1991) Dose-effect approaches to risk assessment. 
    Neurosci Biobehav Rev 15:153-158.
        Glowa, JR; MacPhail, RC. (1995) Qualitative approaches to risk 
    assessment in neurotoxicology. In: Chang, L; Slikker, W, eds. 
    Neurotoxicology: approaches and methods. New York, Academic Press, 
    pp 777-787.
        Goldberg, AM; Frazier, JM. (1989) Alternatives to animals in 
    toxicity testing. Sci Am 261:24-30.
        Goldstein, MK; Stein, GH. (1985) Ambulatory activity in chronic 
    disease. In: Tryon, WH, ed. Behavioral assessment in behavioral 
    medicine. New York: Springer Publishing Co., pp. 160-162.
        Greenfield, SA; Chubb, IW; Grunewald, RA; Henderson, Z; May, J; 
    Protnoy, S; Weston, J; Wright, MC. (1984) A non-cholinergic function 
    for acetylcholinesterase in substantia nigra: behavioral evidence. 
    Exp Brain Res 54:513-520.
        Griffin, JW. (1990) Basic pathologic processes in the nervous 
    system. Toxicol Pathol 18:83-88.
        Hayes, WJ. (1982) Pesticides studied in man. Baltimore: Williams 
    and Wilkins.
        Hughes, JA; Sparber, SB. (1978) d-Amphetamine unmasks postnatal 
    consequences of exposure to methylmercury in utero: methods for 
    studying behavioral teratogenesis. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 8:365-
    375.
        Jacobson, JL; Jacobson, SW. (1996) Prospective, longitudinal 
    assessment of developmental neurotoxicity. Environ Health Perspect 
    104: 275-283.
        Jacobson, SW; Fein, GG; Jacobson, JL; Schwartz, PM; Dowler, JK. 
    (1985) The effect of intrauterine PCB exposure on visual recognition 
    memory. Child Dev 56:853-860.
        Jarabek, AM; Menache, MG; Overton, JH; Dourson, ML; Miller, FJ. 
    (1990) The U.S. Environmental Protection Agency's inhalation RfD 
    methodology: risk assessment for air toxics. Toxicol Ind Health 
    6:279-301.
        Johnson, MK. (1990) Organophosphates and delayed neuropathy: Is 
    NTE alive and well? Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 102:385-399.
        Kimmel, CA; Rees, DC; Francis, EZ, eds. (1990) Qualitative and 
    quantitative comparability of human and animal developmental 
    neurotoxicity. Neurotoxicol Teratol 12:175-292.
        Kimmel, CA; Kavlock, RJ; Francis, EZ. (1992) Animal models for 
    assessing developmental toxicity. In: Guzelian, PS; Henry, CJ; Olin, 
    SS. eds. Similarities and differences between children and adults: 
    implications for risk assessment. Washington, DC: ILSI Press, pp. 
    43-65.
        Kotsonis, FN; Klaassen, CD. (1977) Toxicity and distributions of 
    cadmium administered to rats at sublethal doses. Toxicol Appl 
    Pharmacol 41:667-680.
        Krinke, GJ. (1989) Neuropathologic screening in rodent and other 
    species. J Am Coll Toxicol 8:141-155.
        Landauer, MR; Tomlinson, NT; Balster, RL; MacPhail, RC. (1984) 
    Some effects of the formamidine pesticide chlordimeform on the 
    behavior of mice. Neurotoxicology 5:91-100.
        Last, JM. (1986) Epidemiology and health information. In: Last, 
    JM, ed. Public health and prevention medicine. New York: Appleton-
    Century-Crofts.
        Laties, VG; Evans, HL. (1980) Methylmercury-induced changes in 
    operant discrimination by the pigeon. J Pharmacol Exp Therap 
    214:620-628.
        MacPhail, RC. (1985) Effects of pesticides on schedule-
    controlled behavior. In: Seiden, LS; Balster, RL, eds. Behavioral 
    pharmacology: the current status. New York: A.R. Liss, pp. 519-535.
        MacPhail, RC; Peele, DB; Crofton, KM. (1989) Motor activity and 
    screening for neurotoxicity. J Am Coll Toxicol 8:117-125.
        Mailman, RB. (1987) Mechanisms of CNS injury in behavioral 
    dysfunction. Neurotoxicol Teratol 9:417-426.
        Mattsson, JL; Albee, RR. (1992) Sensory evoked potentials in 
    neurotoxicology. Neurotoxicol Teratol 10:435-443.
        Mattsson, JL; Boyes, WK; Ross, JF. (1992) Incorporating evoked 
    potentials into neurotoxicity test schemes. In: Tilson, HA; 
    Mitchell, CL, eds. Target organ toxicology series: neurotoxicology. 
    New York: Raven Press Ltd., pp. 125-145.
        Mausner, JS; Kramer, S. (1985) Epidemiology: an introductory 
    text, 2nd ed. Philadelphia: WB Saunders.
        Maxwell, DM; Lenz, DE; Groff, WA; Kaminskis, A; Froehlich, HL. 
    (1987) The effects of blood flow and detoxification on in vivo 
    cholinesterase inhibition by Soman in rats. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 
    88:66-76.
        Morell, P; Mailman, RB. (1987) Selective and nonselective 
    effects of organometals on brain neurochemistry. In: Tilson, HA; 
    Sparber, SB, eds. Neurotoxicants and neurobiological function: 
    effects of organoheavy metals. New York: Wiley, pp. 201-230.
        Nagymajtenyi, L; Desi, I; Lorencz, R. (1988) Neurophysiological 
    markers as early signs or organophosphate neurotoxicity. 
    Neurotoxicol Teratol 10:429-434.
        Moser, VC. (1989) Screening approaches to neurotoxicity: a 
    functional observational battery. J Am Coll Toxicol 8:85-93.
        Moser, VC. (1995) Comparisons of the acute effects of 
    cholinesterase inhibitors using a neurobehavioral screening battery 
    in rats. Neurotoxicol Teratol 17:617-625.
        National Research Council (NRC). (1983) Risk assessment in the 
    federal government. Managing the process. Washington, DC: National 
    Academy of Sciences.
        National Research Council (NRC). (1984) Toxicity testing: 
    strategies to determine needs and priorities. Washington, DC: 
    National Academy of Sciences.
        National Research Council (NRC). (1986) Drinking water and 
    health. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences, pp. 173-211.
        National Research Council (NRC). (1994) Science and judgment in 
    risk assessment. Washington, DC: National Academy of Sciences.
        National Research Council (1993). Pesticides in the diets of 
    infants and children. Washington, DC, National Academy of Sciences.
        National Resources Defense Council (NRDC). (1989) Intolerable 
    risk: pesticides in our children's food. New York: Natural Resources 
    Defense Council.
        Needleman, H. (1986) Epidemiological studies. In: Annau, ZA, ed. 
    Neurobehavioral toxicology. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University 
    Press, pp. 279-287.
        Needleman, HL. (1990) Lessons from the history of childhood 
    plumbism for pediatric neurotoxicology. In: Johnson, BL; Anger, WK; 
    Durao, A; Xintaris, C, eds. Advances in neurobehavioral toxicology: 
    application in environmental and occupational health. Chelsea, MI: 
    Lewis Publishers, Inc., pp. 331-337.
        O'Callaghan, JP. (1988) Neurotypic and gliotypic proteins as 
    biochemical markers of neurotoxicity. Neurotoxicol Teratol 10:445-
    452.
        O'Donoghue, JL. (1989) Screening for neurotoxicity using a 
    neurologically based examination and neuropathology. J Am Coll 
    Toxicol 8:97-115.
        Office of Technology Assessment (OTA). (1990) Neurotoxicity: 
    identifying and controlling poisons of the nervous system. U.S. 
    Congress, Office of Technology Assessment (OTA-BA-436). Washington, 
    DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
        Omerand, IE; Harding, AE; Miller, DH; Johnson, G; MacManus, D; 
    duBoulay, EPGH; Kendall, BE; Moseley, IF; McDonald, WI. (1994) 
    Magnetic resonance imaging in degenerative atoxic disorders. J 
    Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 57:51-57.
        Otto, DA. (1992) Assessment of neurobehavioral response in 
    humans to a low level volatile organic compound (VOC) source. Ann. 
    NY Acad Sci 641-248-260.
        Padilla, S; Wilson, VZ; Bushnell, PJ. (1994) Studies on the 
    correlation between blood cholinesterase inhibition and ``tissue'' 
    inhibition in pesticide-treated rats. Toxicology 92:11-25.
        Pope, CN; Chakraborti, TK; Chapman, ML; Farrar, JD. (1992) Long-
    term neurochemical and behavioral effects induced by acute 
    chlorpyrifos treatment. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 42:251-256.
        Porterfield, S. (1994) Vulnerability of the developing brain to 
    thyroid abnormalities: environmental insults to the thyroid system. 
    Environ Health Perspect 102:125-130.
        Rebert, CS. (1983) Multisensory evoked potentials in 
    experimental and applied neurotoxicology. Neurobehav Toxicol Teratol 
    5:659-671.
        Reiter, LW. (1987) Neurotoxicology in regulation and risk 
    assessment. Dev Pharmacol Ther 10:354-368.
        Rees, DC; Hattis, D. (1994) Developing quantitative strategies 
    for animal to human extrapolation. In: Hayes, AW, ed. Principles and 
    methods of toxicology, 3rd ed. New York: Raven Press, pp. 275-315.
        Reuhl, KR. (1991) Delayed expression of neurotoxicity: the 
    problem of silent damage. Neurotoxicology 12:341-346.
        Rice, DC. (1988) Quantification of operant behavior. Toxicol 
    Lett 43:361-379.
    
    [[Page 26952]]
    
        Riley, EP; Vorhees, CV, eds. (1986) Handbook of behavioral 
    teratology. New York: Plenum Press.
        Richardson, RJ. (1995) Assessment of the neurotoxic potential of 
    chlorpyrifos relative to other organophosphorus compounds: a 
    critical review of the literature. J Toxicol Environ Health 44: 135-
    165.
        Rodier, PM. (1978) Behavioral teratology. In: Wilson, JG; 
    Fraser, FC, eds. Handbook of teratology, vol. 4. New York: Plenum 
    Press, pp. 397-428.
        Rodier, P. (1990) Developmental neurotoxicology. Toxicol Pathol 
    18:89-95.
        Salsburg, DS. (1986) Statistics for toxicologists. New York: 
    Marcel Dekker, Inc.
        Sette, WF. (1987) Complexity of neurotoxicological assessment. 
    Neurotoxicol Teratol 9:411-416.
        Sette, WF; MacPhail, RC. (1992) Qualitative and quantitative 
    issues in assessment of neurotoxic effects. In: Tilson, H; Mitchell, 
    C, eds. Target organ toxicity series: Neurotoxicology, 2nd ed. New 
    York: Raven Press, pp. 345-361.
        Siegel, S. (1956) Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral 
    sciences. New York: McGraw-Hill.
        Silbergeld, EK. (1987) Neurochemical approaches to developing 
    markers of neurotoxicity: Review of current status and evaluation of 
    future prospects. Environ Res 63:274-286.
        Silbergeld, EK; Percival, RV. (1987) The organometals: impact of 
    accidental exposure and experimental data on regulatory policies. 
    In: Tilson, HA; Sparber, SB, eds. Neurotoxicants and neurobiological 
    function: effects of organoheavy metals. New York: J. Wiley, pp. 
    328-352.
        Small, DH. (1990) Non-cholinergic actions of 
    acetylcholinesterases: proteases regulating all growth and 
    development? Trends Biol Sci 15:213-216.
        Sokal, RR; Rohlf, FJ. (1969) Biometry. San Francisco: W.H. 
    Freeman and Company.
        Spencer, S; Schaumburg, HH. (1980) Experimental and clinical 
    neurotoxicology. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins.
        Stanton, ME; Spear, LP. (1990) Workshop on the qualitative and 
    quantitative comparability of human and animal developmental 
    neurotoxicity. Workgroup I report: Comparability of measures of 
    developmental neurotoxicity in humans and laboratory animals. 
    Neurotoxicol Teratol 12:261-267.
        Steenland, K; Jenkins, B; Ames, RG; O'Malley, M; Chrislip, D; 
    Russo, J. (1994) Chronic neurological sequelae to organophosphate 
    pesticide poisoning. Am J Public Health 84:731-736.
        Stephens, R; Sprugeon, A; Calvert, IA; Beach, J; Levy, LS; 
    Berry, H; Harrington, JM. (1995) Neurophysiological effects of long-
    term exposure to organophosphates in sheep dip. The Lancet 345:1135-
    1139.
        Sterman, AB; Schaumberg, HH. (1980) The neurological 
    examination. In: Spencer, PS; Schaumberg, HH, eds. Experimental and 
    clinical neurotoxicology. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, pp. 675-
    680.
        Susser, M. (1986) Rules of inference in epidemiology. Regulat 
    Toxicol Pharmacol 6:116-128.
        Tandon, P; Padilla, S; Barone, S; Pope, CN; Tilson, HA. (1994) 
    Fenthion produces a persistent decrease in muscarinic receptor 
    function in the adult rat retina. Toxicol Appl Pharmacol 125:271-
    280.
        Tilson, HA. (1987) Behavioral indices of neurotoxicity: what can 
    be measured? Neurotoxicol Teratol 9:427-443.
        Tilson, HA. (1990) Neurotoxicology in the 1990s. Neurotoxicol 
    Teratol 12:293-300.
        Tilson, HA; Moser, VC. (1992) Comparison of screening 
    approaches. Neurotoxicology 13:1-14.
        Tilson, HA; Mitchell, CL. (1983) Neurotoxicants and adaptive 
    responses of the nervous system. Fed Proc 42:3189-3190.
        Tilson, HA; MacPhail, RC; Crofton, KC. (1995) Defining 
    neurotoxicity in a decision-making context. Neurotoxicology 16:363-
    375.
        Tilson, HA; MacPhail, RC; Crofton, KC. (1996) Setting exposure 
    standards: a decision process. Environ Health Perspect 104:401-405.
        U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. (1986) Triethylene glycol 
    monomethyl, monoethyl, and monobutyl ethers; proposed test rule. 
    Federal Register 51:17883-17894.
        U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. (1987, May 20) Toxic 
    Substances Control Act testing guidelines. 50 FR 39397, September 27, 
    1985, as amended. 40 CFR 798.6050. Federal Register 52:19082.
        U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. (1988a) Diethylene glycol 
    butyl ether and diethylene glycol butyl ether acetate; final test rule. 
    Federal Register 53:5932-5953.
        U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. (1988b) Proposed guidelines 
    for assessing male reproductive risk. Federal Register 53:24850-24869.
        U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. (1989) FIFRA accelerated 
    reregistration phase 3 technical guidance, Appendix D. Office of 
    Prevention, Pesticides and Toxic Substances, Washington, DC. EPA No. 
    540/09-90-078. Available from: NTIS, Springfield, VA. PB-90-161530.
        U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. (1991a) Pesticide assessment 
    guidelines, subdivision F. Hazard evaluation: human and domestic 
    animals. Addendum 10: Neurotoxicity, series 81, 82, and 83. Office of 
    Prevention, Pesticides and Toxic Substances, Washington, DC. EPA 540/
    09-91-123. Available from: NTIS, Springfield, VA. PB91-154617.
        U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. (1991b) Guidelines for 
    developmental toxicity risk assessment. Federal Register 56:63798-
    63826.
        U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. (1992) Guidelines for 
    exposure assessment. Federal Register 57:22888-22938.
        U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. (1994) Final report: 
    principles of neurotoxicology risk assessment. Federal Register 
    59:42360-42404.
        U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. (1995a) The use of the 
    benchmark dose approach in health risk assessment. Office of Research 
    and Development, Washington, DC. EPA/630/R-94/007.
        U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. (1995b) Policy for Risk 
    Characterization. Office of the Administrator, Washington, DC.
        U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. (1995c) Guidance for Risk 
    Characterization. Science Policy Council, Washington, DC.
        U.S. Environmental Protection Agency. (1996) Guidelines for 
    Reproductive Toxicity Risk Assessment. Federal Register 61:56274-56322.
        Valciukas, JA. (1991) Foundations of environmental and occupational 
    neurotoxicology. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.
        Vorhees, CV. (1987) Reliability, sensitivity and validity of 
    indices of neurotoxicity. Neurotoxicol Teratol 9:445-464.
        Winer, BJ. (1971) Statistical principles in experimental design. 
    New York: McGraw-Hill.
        Winneke, G. (1995) Endpoints of developmental neurotoxicity in 
    environmentally exposed children. Toxicol Lett 77:127-136.
        World Health Organization. (1986) Principles and methods for the 
    assessment of neurotoxicity associated with exposure to chemicals. In: 
    Environmental Health Criteria Document 60. Geneva: World Health 
    Organization.
    
    Part B: Response to Science Advisory Board and Public Comments
    
    1. Introduction
    
        A notice of availability for public comments of these Guidelines 
    was published in the Federal Register in October 1995. Twenty-five 
    responses were received. These Guidelines were presented to the 
    Environmental Health Committee of the Science Advisory Board (SAB) on 
    July 18, 1996. The report of the SAB was provided to the Agency in 
    April 1997. The SAB and public comments were diverse and represented 
    varying perspectives. Many of the comments were favorable and expressed 
    agreement with positions taken in the proposed Guidelines. Some 
    comments addressed items that were more pertinent to testing guidance 
    than risk assessment guidance or were otherwise beyond the scope of 
    these Guidelines. Some of the comments concerned generic points that 
    were not specific to neurotoxicity issues. Others
    
    [[Page 26953]]
    
    addressed topics that have not been developed sufficiently and should 
    be viewed as research issues. There were conflicting views about the 
    need to provide additional detailed guidance about decision making in 
    the evaluation process as opposed to promoting extensive use of 
    scientific judgment. Many public comments provided specific suggestions 
    for clarification of details and corrections of factual material in the 
    Guidelines.
    
    2. Response to Science Advisory Board Comments
    
        The SAB found the Guidelines ``* * * to be quite successful, and, 
    all things considered, well suited to its intended task.'' However, 
    recommendations were made to improve specific areas.
        The SAB recommended that EPA keep hazard identification as an 
    identifiable qualitative step in the risk assessment process and that 
    steps should be taken to decouple the qualitative step of hazard 
    identification from the more quantitatively rigorous steps of exposure 
    evaluation and dose-response assessment. These Guidelines now include a 
    hazard characterization step that clearly describes a qualitative 
    evaluation of hazard within the context of the dose, route, timing and 
    duration of exposure. This step is clearly differentiated from the 
    quantitative dose-response analysis, which describes approaches for 
    determining an RfD or RfC.
        The SAB supported the presumption that what appears to be 
    reversible neurotoxicity, especially when arising from gestational or 
    neonatal exposure and observed before adulthood, should not be 
    dismissed as of little practical consequence. They may be indices of 
    silent toxicity that emerge later in life or may suggest more robust 
    and enduring responses in aged individuals. These Guidelines explain 
    the concept of functional reserve and advise caution in instances where 
    reversibility is seen and in cases where exposure to a chemical may 
    result in delayed-onset neurotoxicity. These Guidelines also indicate 
    that reversibility may vary with the region of the nervous system 
    damaged, the neurotoxic agent involved, and organismic factors such as 
    age.
        The SAB restated previous positions concerning cholinesterase-
    inhibiting chemicals. Agent-induced clinical signs of cholinergic 
    dysfunction could be used to evaluate dose-response and dose-effect 
    relationships and define the presence and absence of given effects in 
    risk assessment. The SAB also indicated that inhibition of RBC and 
    plasma cholinesterase activity could serve as a biomarker of exposure 
    to cholinesterase-inhibiting agents and thereby corroborate 
    observations concerning the presence of clinical effects associated 
    with cholinesterase inhibition. The SAB also indicated that reduced 
    brain cholinesterase activity should be assessed in the context of the 
    biological consequences of the reduction. These Guidelines indicate 
    that inhibition of cholinesterase in the nervous system reduces the 
    organism's level of ``reserve'' cholinesterase and, therefore, limits 
    the subsequent ability to respond successfully to additional exposures 
    and that prolonged inhibition could lead to adverse functional changes 
    associated with compensatory neurochemical mechanisms. In general, an 
    attempt was made to coordinate these Guidelines with the views of a 
    recently convened Scientific Advisory Panel regarding the risk 
    assessment of cholinesterase-inhibiting pesticides (Office of Pesticide 
    Programs, Science Policy on the Use of Cholinesterase Inhibition for 
    Risk Assessments of Organophosphate and Carbamate Pesticides, 1997).
        The SAB indicated that the Guidelines were inclusive of the major 
    neurotoxicity endpoints of concern. No additional neurochemical, 
    neurophysiological, or structural endpoints were suggested. Comments 
    indicated that there was no need to consider endocrine disruptors 
    differently from other potential neurotoxic agents.
        The SAB found that the descriptions of the endpoints used in human 
    and animal neurotoxicological assessments were thorough and well 
    documented. Several sections, particularly concerning some of the 
    neurochemical and neurobehavioral measures, were corrected for factual 
    errors or supported with more detailed descriptions.
        The SAB recommended that the use of the threshold assumption should 
    occur after an evaluation of likely biological mechanisms and available 
    data to provide evidence that linear responses would be expected. A 
    strict threshold is not always clear in the human population because of 
    the wide variation in background levels for some functions. Cumulative 
    neurotoxicological effects might also alter the response of some 
    individuals within a special population, which might allow the Agency 
    to characterize the risk to the sensitive population. Although the SAB 
    did not disagree with the Guidelines' assumption of a threshold as a 
    default for neurotoxic effects, it was suggested that the term 
    ``nonlinear dose-response curve for most neurotoxicants'' be 
    substituted for the term ``threshold.'' The Neurotoxicity Risk 
    Assessment Guidelines have been amended to harmonize their treatment of 
    the issue of threshold with the presentation and position taken with 
    other guidelines.
        The SAB also recommended that the topic of susceptible populations 
    be expanded to include the elderly and other groups. The elderly could 
    be at increased risk of toxic effects for a number of reasons, 
    including a decline in the reserve capacity with aging, changes in the 
    ability to detoxify or excrete xenobiotics with age, and the potential 
    to interact with medicines or other compounds that could synergize 
    interactions with toxic chemicals. The SAB also indicated that other 
    populations should be considered, including those with chronic and 
    debilitating conditions, groups of workers with potential exposure to 
    chemicals that may be neurotoxic, individuals with genetic 
    polymorphisms that could affect responsiveness to certain 
    neurotoxicants, and individuals that may experience differential 
    exposure because of their proximity to chemicals in the environment or 
    diet. The Guidelines have been modified to emphasize the possible 
    presence of all of these susceptible populations. When specific 
    information on differential risk is not available, the Agency will 
    continue to apply a default uncertainty factor to account for potential 
    differences in susceptibility.
        The SAB recommended that the benchmark dose (BMD) was not ready for 
    immediate incorporation into adjustment-factor-based safety assessment 
    or to serve as a substitute or replacement for the more familiar NOAEL 
    or LOAEL. The SAB also recommended that research and development on the 
    BMD should be aggressively encouraged and actively supported. The BMD 
    could be a replacement for the NOAEL or LOAEL after the appropriate 
    research has been conducted.
    
    3. Response to Public Comments
    
        In addition to numerous supportive statements, several issues were 
    indicated, although each issue was raised by only a few commentators. 
    The public comment supported the SAB recommendation that there was no 
    clear consensus concerning replacing the NOAEL approach with the BMD to 
    calculate RfDs and RfCs for neurotoxicity endpoints. There was also 
    support for ensuring that dose-response and other experimental design 
    information be considered in interpreting the results of hazard 
    identification studies before proceeding
    
    [[Page 26954]]
    
    to quantitative dose-response analysis. Public comment also supported 
    the position that reversibility cannot be ignored in neurotoxicity risk 
    assessment and that the risk assessor should exert caution in 
    interpreting reversible effects, especially where an apparent transient 
    effect is cited to support evidence for relatively benign effects. The 
    public comment also supported the use of clinical signs in the risk 
    assessment of cholinesterase-inhibiting compounds and the finding that 
    inhibition of brain cholinesterase was an adverse effect. The 
    Guidelines emphasize the importance of brain cholinesterase inhibition, 
    particularly in cases of repeated exposure. The public comment agreed 
    with the SAB that RBC and plasma cholinesterase activity are biomarkers 
    of exposure. It was recommended that the Guidelines incorporate 
    additional information addressing the neuroendocrine system as a 
    potential target site, and a section has been added that defines the 
    vulnerable components of the neuroendocrine system and the behavioral, 
    hormonal, and physiological endpoints that may be indicative of a 
    direct or indirect effect on the neuroendocrine system.
        Public comment strongly endorsed the default assumption that there 
    is a threshold for neurotoxic effects. The Guidelines, however, reflect 
    the argument of the SAB that the term ``nonlinear dose-response curve 
    for most neurotoxicants'' be substituted for ``threshold'' in order to 
    be consistent with the presentation and positions taken by other risk 
    assessment guidelines.
        The public comments made a number of recommendations to improve the 
    Guidelines with regard to consistency of language between text and 
    tables, improve the clarity of some of the tables, and improve the 
    description of some of the endpoints used in animal studies. A number 
    of factual errors were corrected, including the description of the 
    blood-brain barrier and the degree of inhibition of neurotoxic esterase 
    associated with organophosphate-induced delayed-onset neuropathy. 
    Therefore, a number of changes have been made in the Guidelines to 
    clarify and correct specific passages, but every effort was made to 
    maintain the original intent concerning the use and interpretation of 
    results from various neurotoxicological endpoints. Finally, the public 
    comment agreed with the SAB that factors such as nutrition, personal 
    habits, age, or preexisting disease may predispose some individuals to 
    be differentially sensitive to neurotoxic chemicals. The risk 
    characterization section has been expanded to reflect these potentially 
    sensitive subpopulations.
    
    [FR Doc. 98-12303 Filed 5-13-98; 8:45 am]
    BILLING CODE 6560-50-P
    
    
    

Document Information

Effective Date:
4/30/1998
Published:
05/14/1998
Department:
Environmental Protection Agency
Entry Type:
Notice
Action:
Notice of availability of final Guidelines for Neurotoxicity Risk Assessment.
Document Number:
98-12303
Dates:
The Guidelines will be effective on April 30, 1998.
Pages:
26926-26954 (29 pages)
Docket Numbers:
FRL-6011-3
RINs:
2080-AA08: Guidelines for Neurotoxicity Risk Assessment
RIN Links:
https://www.federalregister.gov/regulations/2080-AA08/guidelines-for-neurotoxicity-risk-assessment
PDF File:
98-12303.pdf